Motivation, Wikipedia

#has-images

Motivation

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Jump to navigationJump to search

"Motivate" redirects here. For the bike-sharing company, see Motivate (company).

For other uses, see Motivation (disambiguation).

hideThis article has multiple issues. Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page. (Learn how and when to remove these template messages)

The lead section of this article may need to be rewritten. (November 2019)

This article's tone or style may not reflect the encyclopedic tone used on Wikipedia. (February 2020)

This article needs additional citations for verification. (February 2020)

Motivation is the experience of desire or aversion (you want something, or want to avoid or escape something). As such, motivation has both an objective aspect (a goal or thing you aspire to) and an internal or subjective aspect (it is you that wants the thing or wants it to go away).

At minimum, motivation requires the biological substrate for physical sensations of pleasure and pain; animals can thus want or disdain specific objects based on sense perception and experience. Motivation goes on to include the capacity to form concepts and to reason, which allows humans to be able to surpass this minimum state, with a much greater possible range of desires and aversions. This much greater range is supported by the ability to choose one's own goals and values, combined with "time horizons" for value achievement that can perhaps encompass years, decades, or longer, and the ability to re-experience past events.[1] Some models treat as important the distinction between extrinsic and intrinsic motivation,[2][3] and motivation is an important topic in work,[4] organizational psychology, administrative organization, management,[5] as well as education.

The definition of motivation as experienced desires and aversions highlights the association of motivation with emotion. It is believed that emotions are automatic appraisals based on subconsciously stored values and beliefs about the object. To the extent that distinct emotions relate to specific subconscious appraisals (e.g., anger—injustice; guilt—violation of a moral standard; sadness—loss of a value; pride—the achievement of a moral ideal; love—valuing an object or person; joy—the attainment of an important value; envy—wanting the attainments of another, admiration—valuing the attainments of another, etc.), motivation theory involves specifying "content theories"—values that people find motivating—along with mechanisms by which they might attain these values (mastery, setting challenging goals, attending to required tasks, persistence, etc).

Changing motivation—either one's own or that of others (e.g., employees)—is another focus of motivation research (for example, altering how you choose to act on your emotions and re-programming them by modifying one's beliefs and values).[6]

Contents

Neuroscience[edit]

Main article: Motivational salience

Motivation as a desire to perform an action is usually defined as having two parts: directional (such as directed towards a positive stimulus or away from a negative one), as well as the activated "seeking phase" and consummatory "liking phase". This type of motivation has neurobiological roots in the basal ganglia and mesolimbic (dopaminergic) pathways. Activated "seeking" behaviour, such as locomotor activity, is influenced by dopaminergic drugs, and microdialysis experiments reveal that dopamine is released during the anticipation of a reward.[7] The "wanting behaviour" associated with a rewarding stimulus can be increased by microinjections of dopamine and dopaminergic drugs in the dorsorostral nucleus accumbens and posterior ventral palladum. Opioid injections in this area produce pleasure; however, outside of these hedonic hotspots they create an increased desire.[8] Furthermore, depletion or inhibition of dopamine in neurons of the nucleus accumbens decreases appetitive but not consummatory behaviour. Dopamine is further implicated in motivation as administration of amphetamine increased the break point in a progressive ratio self-reinforcement schedule. That is, subjects were willing to go to greater lengths (e.g. press a lever more times) to obtain a reward.[9]

Psychological theories[edit]

Motivation can be conceived of as a cycle in which thoughts influence behaviours, drive performance affects thoughts, and the cycle begins again. Each stage of the cycle is composed of many dimensions including attitudes, beliefs, intentions, effort, and withdrawal, which can all affect the motivation that an individual experiences. Most psychological theories hold that motivation exists purely within the individual, but socio-cultural theories express motivation as an outcome of participation in actions and activities within the cultural context of social groups.[10]

Content theories[edit]

Theories articulating the content of motivation: what kinds of thing people find motivating are among the earliest theories in the history of motivation research. Because content theories focus on which categories of goal (needs) motivate people, content theories are related to need theories.

Maslow's hierarchy of needs[edit]

Maslow's hierarchy of needs, represented as a pyramid with the more basic needs at the bottom

Content theory of human motivation includes both Abraham Maslow's hierarchy of needs and Herzberg's two-factor theory. Maslow's theory is one of the most widely discussed theories of motivation. Abraham Maslow believed that man is inherently good and argued that individuals possess a constantly growing inner drive that has great potential. The needs hierarchy system is a commonly used scheme for classifying human motives.[11]

The American motivation psychologist Abraham H. Maslow (1954) developed the hierarchy of needs consisting of five hierarchic classes. According to Maslow, people are motivated by unsatisfied needs. The needs, listed from basic (lowest-earliest) to most complex (highest-latest) are as follows:[12]

The basic requirements build upon the first step in the pyramid: physiology. If there are deficits on this level, all behavior will be oriented to satisfy this deficit. Essentially, if you have not slept or eaten adequately, you won't be interested in your self-esteem desires. Subsequently, we have the second level, which awakens a need for security. After securing those two levels, the motives shift to the social sphere, the third level. Psychological requirements comprise the fourth level, while the top of the hierarchy consists of self-realization and self-actualization.

Maslow's hierarchy of needs theory can be summarized as follows:

  • Human beings have wants and desires which, when unsatisfied, may influence behavior.
  • Differing levels of importance to human life are reflected in a hierarchical structure of needs.
  • Needs at higher levels in the hierarchy are held in abeyance until lower level needs are at least minimally satisfied.
  • Needs at higher levels of the hierarchy are associated with individuality, humanness and psychological health.

Sex, Hedonism, and Evolution[edit]

One of the first influential figures to discuss the topic of hedonism was Socrates, and he did so around 470–399 BCE in ancient Greece. Hedonism, as Socrates described it, is the motivation wherein a person will behave in a manner that will maximize pleasure and minimize pain. The only instance in which a person will behave in a manner that results in more pain than pleasure is when the knowledge of the effects of the behavior is lacking. Sex is one of the pleasures people pursue.[13][14]

Sex is on the first level of Maslow's hierarchy of needs. It is a necessary physiological need like air, warmth, or sleep, and if the body lacks it will not function optimally. Without the orgasm that comes with sex, a person will experience “pain,” and as hedonism would predict, a person will minimize this pain by pursuing sex. That being said, sex as a basic need is different from the need for sexual intimacy, which is located on the third level in Maslow's hierarchy.[13]

There are multiple theories for why sex is a strong motivation, and many fall under the theory of evolution. On an evolutionary level, the motivation for sex likely has to do with a species’ ability to reproduce. Species that reproduce more, survive and pass on their genes. Therefore, species have sexual desire that leads to sexual intercourse as a means to create more offspring. Without this innate motivation, a species may determine that attaining intercourse is too costly in terms of effort, energy, and danger.[13][15]

In addition to sexual desire, the motivation for romantic love runs parallel in having an evolutionary function for the survival of a species. On an emotional level, romantic love satiates a psychological need for belonging. Therefore, this is another hedonistic pursuit of pleasure. From the evolutionary perspective, romantic love creates bonds with the parents of offspring. This bond will make it so that the parents will stay together and take care and protect the offspring until it is independent. By rearing the child together, it increases the chances that the offspring will survive and pass on its genes itself, therefore continuing the survival of the species. Without the romantic love bond, the male will pursue satiation of his sexual desire with as many mates as possible, leaving behind the female to rear the offspring by herself. Child rearing with one parent is more difficult and provides less assurance that the offspring survives than with two parents. Romantic love therefore solves the commitment problem of parents needing to be together; individuals that are loyal and faithful to one another will have mutual survival benefits.[13][16][17]

Additionally, under the umbrella of evolution, is Darwin's term sexual selection. This refers to how the female selects the male for reproduction. The male is motivated to attain sex because of all the aforementioned reasons, but how he attains it can vary based on his qualities. For some females, they are motivated by the will to survive mostly, and will prefer a mate that can physically defend her, or financially provide for her (among humans). Some females are more attracted to charm, as it is an indicator of being a good loyal lover that will in turn make for a dependable child rearing partner. Altogether, sex is a hedonistic pleasure seeking behavior that satiates physical and psychological needs and is instinctively guided by principles of evolution.[13][18]

Herzberg's two-factor theory[edit]

Main article: Two-factor theory

Two-factor theory

Frederick Herzberg's two-factor theory concludes that certain factors in the workplace result in job satisfaction (motivators), while others (hygiene factors), if absent, lead to dissatisfaction but are not related to satisfaction. The name hygiene factors is used because, like hygiene, the presence will not improve health, but absence can cause health deterioration.

The factors that motivate people can change over their lifetime. Some claimed motivating factors (satisfiers) were: Achievement, recognition, work itself, responsibility, advancement, and growth. Some hygiene factors (dissatisfiers) were: company policy, supervision, working conditions, interpersonal relations, salary, status, job security, and personal life.[19]

Alderfer's ERG theory[edit]

Main article: ERG theory

Alderfer, building on Maslow's hierarchy of needs, posited that needs identified by Maslow exist in three groups of core needs — existence, relatedness, and growth, hence the label: ERG theory. The existence group is concerned with providing our basic material existence requirements. They include the items that Maslow considered to be physiological and safety needs. The second group of needs are those of relatedness- the desire we have for maintaining important personal relationships. These social and status desires require interaction with others if they are to be satisfied, and they align with Maslow's social need and the external component of Maslow's esteem classification. Finally, Alderfer isolates growth needs as an intrinsic desire for personal development. All these needs should be fulfilled to greater wholeness as a human being.[20]

Self-determination theory[edit]

Main article: Self-determination theory

Since the early 1970s Deci[21] and Ryan have developed and tested their self-determination theory (SDT). SDT identifies three innate needs that, if satisfied, allow optimal function and growth: competence,[22][23] relatedness,[24] and autonomy.[25][26] These three psychological needs are suggested to be essential for psychological health and well-being and to motivate behaviour.[27] There are three essential elements to the theory:[28]

  • Humans are inherently proactive with their potential and mastering their inner forces (such as drive and emotions).
  • Humans have an inherent tendency towards growth, development and integrated functioning.
  • Optimal development and actions are inherent in humans but they do not happen automatically.

Within Self-Determination Theory, Deci & Ryan[29] distinguish between four different types of extrinsic motivation, differing in their levels of perceived autonomy:

  • External regulation: This is the least autonomous of the four and is determined by external punishment or reward.
  • Introjected regulation: This form of external motivation arises when the individual has somewhat internalized regulations but does not fully accept them as their own. They may comply for self-esteem reasons or social acceptability - essentially internal reasons but externally driven.
  • Identified regulation: This more autonomously driven - when the individual consciously perceives the actions as valuable.
  • Integrated regulation: This is the most autonomous form of motivation and the action has been internalized and is aligned with the individual's values, beliefs and is perceived as necessary for their wellbeing. However this is still classified as extrinsic motivation as it is still driven by external processes and not by inherent enjoyment for the task itself.

"16 basic desires" theory[edit]

Starting from studies involving more than 6,000 people, Reiss proposed that 16 basic desires guide nearly all human behavior.[30] In this model the basic desires that motivate our actions and define our personalities are:

Natural theories[edit]

The natural system assumes that people have higher order needs, which contrasts with the rational theory that suggests people dislike work and only respond to rewards and punishment.[31] According to McGregor's Theory Y, human behaviour is based on satisfying a hierarchy of needs: physiological, safety, social, ego, and self-fulfillment.[32]

Physiological needs are the lowest and most important level. These fundamental requirements include food, rest, shelter, and exercise. After physiological needs are satisfied, employees can focus on safety needs, which include “protection against danger, threat, deprivation.”[32] However, if management makes arbitrary or biased employment decisions, then an employee's safety needs are unfulfilled.

The next set of needs is social, which refers to the desire for acceptance, affiliation, reciprocal friendships and love. As such, the natural system of management assumes that close-knit work teams are productive. Accordingly, if an employee's social needs are unmet, then he will act disobediently.[32]

There are two types of egoistic needs, the second-highest order of needs. The first type refers to one's self-esteem, which encompasses self-confidence, independence, achievement, competence, and knowledge. The second type of needs deals with reputation, status, recognition, and respect from colleagues.[32] Egoistic needs are much more difficult to satisfy.

The highest order of needs is for self-fulfillment, including recognition of one's full potential, areas for self-improvement, and the opportunity for creativity. This differs from the rational system, which assumes that people prefer routine and security to creativity.[31] Unlike the rational management system, which assumes that humans don't care about these higher order needs, the natural system is based on these needs as a means for motivation.

The author of the reductionist motivation model is Sigmund Freud. According to the model, physiological needs raise tension, thereby forcing an individual to seek an outlet by satisfying those needs Ziegler, Daniel (1992). Personality Theories: Basic Assumptions, Research, and Applications.

Self-management through teamwork[edit]

To successfully manage and motivate employees, the natural system posits that being part of a group is necessary.[33] Because of structural changes in social order, the workplace is more fluid and adaptive according to Mayo. As a result, individual employees have lost their sense of stability and security, which can be provided by a membership in a group. However, if teams continuously change within jobs, then employees feel anxious, empty, and irrational and become harder to work with.[33] The innate desire for lasting human association and management “is not related to single workers, but always to working groups.”[33] In groups, employees will self-manage and form relevant customs, duties, and traditions.

Wage incentives[edit]

Humans are motivated by additional factors besides wage incentives.[34] Unlike the rational theory of motivation, people are not driven toward economic interests per the natural system. For instance, the straight piecework system pays employees based on each unit of their output. Based on studies such as the Bank Wiring Observation Room, using a piece rate incentive system does not lead to higher production.[34] Employees actually set upper limits on each person's daily output. These actions stand “in direct opposition to the ideas underlying their system of financial incentive, which countenanced no upper limit to performance other than physical capacity.”[34] Therefore, as opposed to the rational system that depends on economic rewards and punishments, the natural system of management assumes that humans are also motivated by non-economic factors.

Autonomy: increased motivation for autonomous tasks[edit]

Employees seek autonomy and responsibility in their work, contrary to assumptions of the rational theory of management. Because supervisors have direct authority over employees, they must ensure that the employee's actions are in line with the standards of efficient conduct.[34] This creates a sense of restriction on the employee and these constraints are viewed as “annoying and seemingly functioned only as subordinating or differentiating mechanisms."[34] Accordingly, the natural management system assumes that employees prefer autonomy and responsibility on the job and dislike arbitrary rules and overwhelming supervision. An individual's motivation to complete a task is increased when this task is autonomous. When the motivation to complete a task comes from an "external pressure" that pressure then "undermines" a person's motivation, and as a result decreases a persons desire to complete the task.[35]

Rational motivations[edit]

The idea that human beings are rational and human behaviour is guided by reason is an old one. However, recent research (on satisfying for example) has significantly undermined the idea of homo economicus or of perfect rationality in favour of a more bounded rationality. The field of behavioural economics is particularly concerned with the limits of rationality in economic agents.[36]

Incentive theories: intrinsic and extrinsic motivation[edit]

Motivation can be divided into two different theories known as intrinsic (internal or inherent) motivation and extrinsic (external) motivation.

Intrinsic motivation[edit]

See also: Ikigai

Intrinsic motivation has been studied since the early 1970s. Intrinsic motivation is a behavior that is driven by satisfying internal rewards. For example, an athlete may enjoy playing football for the experience, rather than for an award. [2] It is an interest or enjoyment in the task itself, and exists within the individual rather than relying on external pressures or a desire for consideration. Deci (1971) explained that some activities provide their own inherent reward, meaning certain activities are not dependent on external rewards.[37] The phenomenon of intrinsic motivation was first acknowledged within experimental studies of animal behaviour. In these studies, it was evident that the organisms would engage in playful and curiosity-driven behaviours in the absence of reward. Intrinsic motivation is a natural motivational tendency and is a critical element in cognitive, social, and physical development.[38] The two necessary elements for intrinsic motivation are self-determination and an increase in perceived competence.[39] In short, the cause of the behaviour must be internal, known as internal locus of causality, and the individual who engages in the behaviour must perceive that the task increases their competence.[38] According to various research reported by Deci's published findings in 1971, and 1972, tangible rewards could actually undermine the intrinsic motivation of college students. However, these studies didn't just affect college students, Kruglanski, Friedman and Zeevi (1971) repeated this study and found that symbolic and material rewards can undermine not just high school students, but preschool students as well.

Students who are intrinsically motivated are more likely to engage in the task willingly as well as work to improve their skills, which will increase their capabilities.[40] Students are likely to be intrinsically motivated if they...

  • attribute their educational results to factors under their own control, also known as autonomy or locus of control
  • believe they have the skills to be effective agents in reaching their desired goals, also known as self-efficacy beliefs
  • are interested in mastering a topic, not just in achieving good grades
  • don't act from pressure, but from interest

An example of intrinsic motivation is when an employee becomes an IT professional because he or she wants to learn about how computer users interact with computer networks. The employee has the intrinsic motivation to gain more knowledge, and will continue to want to learn even in the face of failure.[41] Art for art's sake is an example of intrinsic motivation in the domain of art.

Traditionally, researchers thought of motivations to use computer systems to be primarily driven by extrinsic purposes; however, many modern systems have their use driven primarily by intrinsic motivations.[42] Examples of such systems used primarily to fulfill users' intrinsic motivations, include on-line gaming, virtual worlds, online shopping,[43] learning/education, online dating, digital music repositories, social networking, online pornography, gamified systems, and general gamification. Even traditional management information systems (e.g., ERP, CRM) are being 'gamified' such that both extrinsic and intrinsic motivations must increasingly be considered. Deci's findings didn't come without controversy. Articles stretching over the span of 25 years from the perspective of behavioral theory argue there isn't enough evidence to explain intrinsic motivation and this theory would inhibit "scientific progress." As stated above, we now can see technology such as various forms of computer systems are highly intrinsic.[37]

Not only can intrinsic motivation be used in a personal setting, but it can also be implemented and utilized in a social environment. Instead of attaining mature desires, such as those presented above via internet which can be attained on one's own, intrinsic motivation can be used to assist extrinsic motivation to attain a goal. For example, Eli, a 4-year-old with autism, wants to achieve the goal of playing with a toy train.[44] To get the toy, he must first communicate to his therapist that he wants it. His desire to play is strong enough to be considered intrinsic motivation because it is a natural feeling, and his desire to communicate with his therapist to get the train can be considered extrinsic motivation because the outside object is a reward (see incentive theory). Communicating with the therapist is the first, slightly more challenging goal that stands in the way of achieving his larger goal of playing with the train. Achieving these goals in attainable pieces is also known as the goal-setting theory. The three elements of goal-setting (STD) are Specific, Time-bound, and Difficult. Specifically goals should be set in the 90th percentile of difficulty. [10]

Intrinsic motivation comes from one's desire to achieve or attain a goal. [2]Pursuing challenges and goals come easier and more enjoyable when one is intrinsically motivated to complete a certain objective because the individual is more interested in learning, rather than achieving the goal.[3] Edward Deci and Richard Ryan's theory of intrinsic motivation is essentially examining the conditions that “elicit and sustain” this phenomenon.[45] Deci and Ryan coin the term “cognitive evaluation theory which concentrates on the needs of competence and autonomy. The CET essentially states that social-contextual events like feedback and reinforcement can cause feelings of competence and therefore increase intrinsic motivation. However, feelings of competence will not increase intrinsic motivation if there is no sense of autonomy. In situations where choices, feelings, and opportunities are present, intrinsic motivation is increased because people feel a greater sense of autonomy.[45] Offering people choices, responding to their feelings, and opportunities for self-direction have been reported to enhance intrinsic motivation via increased autonomy (Deci & Ryan, 1985).[46][47]

An advantage (relative to extrinsic motivation) is that intrinsic motivators can be long-lasting, self-sustaining, and satisfying. [3] For this reason, efforts in education sometimes attempt to modify intrinsic motivation with the goal of promoting future student learning performance, creativity, and learning via long-term modifications in interests. [2] Intrinsic motivators are suggested[by whom?] to involve increased feelings of reward and thus may support subjective well-being.[citation needed] By contrast, intrinsic motivation has been found to be hard to modify, and attempts to recruit existing intrinsic motivators require a non-trivially difficult individualized approach, identifying and making relevant the different motivators of needed to motivate different students [2], possibly requiring additional skills and intrinsic motivation from the instructor.[48]

Extrinsic motivation[edit]

See also: Goal orientation

Extrinsic motivation comes from influences outside of the individual. In extrinsic motivation, the harder question to answer is where do people get the motivation to carry out and continue to push with persistence. Usually extrinsic motivation is used to attain outcomes that a person wouldn't get from intrinsic motivation.[3] Common extrinsic motivations are rewards (for example money or grades) for showing the desired behaviour, and the threat of punishment following misbehaviour. Competition is an extrinsic motivator because it encourages the performer to win and to beat others, not simply to enjoy the intrinsic rewards of the activity. A cheering crowd and the desire to win a trophy are also extrinsic incentives.[49] For example, if an individual plays the sport tennis to receive an award, that would be extrinsic motivation. VS. The individual play because he or she enjoys the game, that would be intrinsic motivation. [2]

The most simple distinction between extrinsic and intrinsic motivation is the type of reasons or goals that lead to an action. While intrinsic motivation refers to doing something because it is inherently interesting or enjoyable and satisfying, extrinsic motivation, refers to doing something because it leads to a separable outcome.[3] Extrinsic motivation thus contrasts with intrinsic motivation, which is doing an activity simply for the enjoyment of the activity itself, instead of for its instrumental value.[2]

Social psychological research has indicated that extrinsic rewards can lead to overjustification and a subsequent reduction in intrinsic motivation. In one study demonstrating this effect, children who expected to be (and were) rewarded with a ribbon and a gold star for drawing pictures spent less time playing with the drawing materials in subsequent observations than children who were assigned to an unexpected reward condition.[50] This shows how if an individual expects an award they don't care about the outcome. VS. if an individual doesn't expect a reward they will care more about the task. [3]However, another study showed that third graders who were rewarded with a book showed more reading behaviour in the future, implying that some rewards do not undermine intrinsic motivation.[51] While the provision of extrinsic rewards might reduce the desirability of an activity, the use of extrinsic constraints, such as the threat of punishment, against performing an activity has actually been found to increase one's intrinsic interest in that activity. In one study, when children were given mild threats against playing with an attractive toy, it was found that the threat actually served to increase the child's interest in the toy, which was previously undesirable to the child in the absence of threat.[52]

Advantages of extrinsic motivators are that they easily promote motivation to work and persist to goal completion. Rewards are tangible and beneficial. [3] A disadvantage for extrinsic motivators relative to internal is that work does not persist long once external rewards are removed. As the task is completed for the reward quality of work may need to be monitored [2], and it has been suggested that extrinsic motivators may diminish in value over time. [3]

Flow theory[edit]

Flow theory refers to desirable subjective state a person experiences when completely involved in some challenging activity that matches the individual skill.[53]

Mihali Csikszentmihaly described Flow theory as "A state in which people are so involved in an activity that nothing else seems to matter; the experience is so enjoyable that people will continue to do it even at great cost, for the sheer sake of doing it."[54]

The idea of flow theory as first conceptualized by Csikszentmihalyi. Flow in the context of motivation can be seen as an activity that is not too hard, frustrating or madding, or too easy boring and done too fast. If one has achieved perfect flow, then the activity has reached maximum potential.[54]

Flow is part of something called positive psychology of the psychology of happiness. Positive psychology looks into what makes a person happy. Flow can be considered as achieving happiness or at the least positive feelings. A study that was published in the journal Emotion looked at flow experienced in college students playing Tetris. The students that they were being evaluated on looks then told to wait and play Tetris. There were three categories; Easy, normal, and hard. The students that played Tetris on normal level experienced flow and were less stressed about the evaluation.[55]

Csikszentmihalyi describes 8 characteristics of flow as a complete concentration on the task, clarity of goals and reward in mind and immediate feedback, transformation of time (speeding up/slowing down of time), the experience is intrinsically rewarding, effortlessness and ease, there is a balance between challenge and skills, actions and awareness are merged, losing self-conscious rumination, there is a feeling of control over the task.[54]

The activity no longer becomes something seen as a means to an end and it becomes something an individual wants to do. This can be seen as someone who likes to run for the sheer joy of running and not because they need to do it for exercise or because they want to brag about it. Peak flow can be different for each person. It could take an individual years to reach flow or only moments. If an individual becomes too good at an activity they can become bored. If the challenge becomes too hard then the individual could become discouraged and want to quit.[56]

Behaviorist theories[edit]

While many theories on motivation have a mentalistic perspective, behaviorists focus only on observable behaviour and theories founded on experimental evidence. In the view of behaviorism, motivation is understood as a question about what factors cause, prevent, or withhold various behaviours, while the question of, for instance, conscious motives would be ignored. Where others would speculate about such things as values, drives, or needs, that may not be observed directly, behaviorists are interested in the observable variables that affect the type, intensity, frequency and duration of observable behaviour. Through the basic research of such scientists as Pavlov, Watson and Skinner, several basic mechanisms that govern behaviour have been identified. The most important of these are classical conditioning and operand conditioning.

Classical and operant conditioning[edit]

Main article: Motivational salience

In classical (or respondent) conditioning, behaviour is understood as responses triggered by certain environmental or physical stimuli. They can be unconditioned, such as in-born reflexes, or learned through the pairing of an unconditioned stimulus with a different stimulus, which then becomes a conditioned stimulus. In relation to motivation, classical conditioning might be seen as one explanation as to why an individual performs certain responses and behaviors in certain situations.[57][58] For instance, a dentist might wonder why a patient does not seem motivated to show up for an appointment, with the explanation being that the patient has associated the dentist (conditioned stimulus) with the pain (unconditioned stimulus) that elicits a fear response (conditioned response), leading to the patient being reluctant to visit the dentist.

In operant conditioning, the type and frequency of behaviour is determined mainly by its consequences. If a certain behaviour, in the presence of a certain stimulus, is followed by a desirable consequence (a reinforcer), the emitted behaviour will increase in frequency in the future, in the presence of the stimulus that preceded the behaviour (or a similar one). Conversely, if the behaviour is followed by something undesirable (a punisher), the behaviour is less likely to occur in the presence of the stimulus. In a similar manner, removal of a stimulus directly following the behaviour might either increase or decrease the frequency of that behaviour in the future (negative reinforcement or punishment).[57][58] For instance, a student that gained praise and a good grade after turning in a paper, might seem more motivated in writing papers in the future (positive reinforcement); if the same student put in a lot of work on a task without getting any praise for it, he or she might seem less motivated to do school work in the future (negative punishment). If a student starts to cause trouble in class gets punished with something he or she dislikes, such as detention (positive punishment), that behaviour would decrease in the future. The student might seem more motivated to behave in class, presumably in order to avoid further detention (negative reinforcement).

The strength of reinforcement or punishment is dependent on schedule and timing. A reinforcer or punisher affects the future frequency of a behaviour most strongly if it occurs within seconds of the behaviour. A behaviour that is reinforced intermittently, at unpredictable intervals, will be more robust and persistent, compared to one that is reinforced every time the behaviour is performed.[57][58] For example, if the misbehaving student in the above example was punished a week after the troublesome behaviour, that might not affect future behaviour.

In addition to these basic principles, environmental stimuli also affect behavior. Behaviour is punished or reinforced in the context of whatever stimuli were present just before the behaviour was performed, which means that a particular behaviour might not be affected in every environmental context, or situation, after it is punished or reinforced in one specific context.[57][58] A lack of praise for school-related behaviour might, for instance, not decrease after-school sports-related behaviour that is usually reinforced by praise.

The various mechanisms of operant conditioning may be used to understand the motivation for various behaviours by examining what happens just after the behaviour (the consequence), in what context the behaviour is performed or not performed (the antecedent), and under what circumstances (motivating operators).[57][58]

Incentive motivation[edit]

Main section: Motivational salience § Incentive salience

Incentive theory is a specific theory of motivation, derived partly from behaviorist principles of reinforcement, which concerns an incentive or motive to do something. The most common incentive would be a compensation. Compensation can be tangible or intangible, It helps in motivating the employees in their corporate life, students in academics and inspire to do more and more to achieve profitability in every field. Studies show that if the person receives the reward immediately, the effect is greater, and decreases as delay lengthens.[citation needed] Repetitive action-reward combination can cause the action to become a habit[citation needed]

"Reinforcers and reinforcement principles of behaviour differ from the hypothetical construct of reward." A reinforcer is anything that follows an action, with the intentions that the action will now occur more frequently. From this perspective, the concept of distinguishing between intrinsic and extrinsic forces is irrelevant.

Incentive theory in psychology treats motivation and behaviour of the individual as they are influenced by beliefs, such as engaging in activities that are expected to be profitable. Incentive theory is promoted by behavioral psychologists, such as B.F. Skinner. Incentive theory is especially supported by Skinner in his philosophy of Radical behaviorism, meaning that a person's actions always have social ramifications: and if actions are positively received people are more likely to act in this manner, or if negatively received people are less likely to act in this manner.

Incentive theory distinguishes itself from other motivation theories, such as drive theory, in the direction of the motivation. In incentive theory, stimuli "attract" a person towards them, and push them towards the stimulus. In terms of behaviorism, incentive theory involves positive reinforcement: the reinforcing stimulus has been conditioned to make the person happier. As opposed to in drive theory, which involves negative reinforcement: a stimulus has been associated with the removal of the punishment—the lack of homeostasis in the body. For example, a person has come to know that if they eat when hungry, it will eliminate that negative feeling of hunger, or if they drink when thirsty, it will eliminate that negative feeling of thirst.[citation needed]

Motivating operations[edit]

Motivating operations, MOs, relate to the field of motivation in that they help improve understanding aspects of behaviour that are not covered by operant conditioning. In operant conditioning, the function of the reinforcer is to influence future behavior. The presence of a stimulus believed to function as a reinforcer does not according to this terminology explain the current behaviour of an organism – only previous instances of reinforcement of that behavior (in the same or similar situations) do. Through the behavior-altering effect of MOs, it is possible to affect current behaviour of an individual, giving another piece of the puzzle of motivation.

Motivating operations are factors that affect learned behaviour in a certain context. MOs have two effects: a value-altering effect, which increases or decreases the efficiency of a reinforcer, and a behavior-altering effect, which modifies learned behaviour that has previously been punished or reinforced by a particular stimulus.[57]

When a motivating operation causes an increase in the effectiveness of a reinforcer, or amplifies a learned behaviour in some way (such as increasing frequency, intensity, duration or speed of the behaviour), it functions as an establishing operation, EO. A common example of this would be food deprivation, which functions as an EO in relation to food: the food-deprived organism will perform behaviours previously related to the acquisition of food more intensely, frequently, longer, or faster in the presence of food, and those behaviours would be especially strongly reinforced.[57] For instance, a fast-food worker earning minimal wage, forced to work more than one job to make ends meet, would be highly motivated by a pay raise, because of the current deprivation of money (a conditioned establishing operation). The worker would work hard to try to achieve the raise, and getting the raise would function as an especially strong reinforcer of work behaviour.

Conversely, a motivating operation that causes a decrease in the effectiveness of a reinforcer, or diminishes a learned behaviour related to the reinforcer, functions as an abolishing operation, AO. Again using the example of food, satiation of food prior to the presentation of a food stimulus would produce a decrease on food-related behaviours, and diminish or completely abolish the reinforcing effect of acquiring and ingesting the food.[57] Consider the board of a large investment bank, concerned with a too small profit margin, deciding to give the CEO a new incentive package in order to motivate him to increase firm profits. If the CEO already has a lot of money, the incentive package might not be a very good way to motivate him, because he would be satiated on money. Getting even more money wouldn't be a strong reinforcer for profit-increasing behaviour, and wouldn't elicit increased intensity, frequency or duration of profit-increasing behaviour.

Motivation and psychotherapy[edit]

See also: Motivational interviewing

Motivation lies at the core of many behaviorist approaches to psychological treatment. A person with autism-spectrum disorder is seen as lacking motivation to perform socially relevant behaviours – social stimuli are not as reinforcing for people with autism compared to other people. Depression is understood as a lack of reinforcement (especially positive reinforcement) leading to extinction of behavior in the depressed individual. A patient with specific phobia is not motivated to seek out the phobic stimulus because it acts as a punisher, and is over-motivated to avoid it (negative reinforcement). In accordance, therapies have been designed to address these problems, such as EIBI and CBT for major depression and specific phobia.

Socio-cultural theory[edit]

See also: Cultural-historical psychology

Sociocultural theory (also known as Social Motivation) emphasizes impact of activity and actions mediated through social interaction, and within social contexts. Sociocultural theory represents a shift from traditional theories of motivation, which view the individual's innate drives or mechanistic operand learning as primary determinants of motivation. Critical elements to socio-cultural theory applied to motivation include, but are not limited to, the role of social interactions and the contributions from culturally-based knowledge and practice.[10] Sociocultural theory extends the social aspects of Cognitive Evaluation Theory, which espouses the important role of positive feedback from others during action,[3] but requires the individual as the internal locus of causality. Sociocultural theory predicts that motivation has an external locus of causality, and is socially distributed among the social group.[10]

Motivation can develop through an individuals involvement within their cultural group. Personal motivation often comes from activities a person believes to be central to the everyday occurrences in their community.[59] An example of socio-cultural theory would be social settings where people work together to solve collective problems. Although individuals will have internalized goals, they will also develop internalized goals of others, as well as new interests and goals collectively with those that they feel socially connected to.[60] Oftentimes, it is believed that all cultural groups are motivated in the same way. However, motivation can come from different child-rearing practices and cultural behaviors that greatly vary between cultural groups.

In some indigenous cultures, collaboration between children and adults in community and household tasks is seen as very important [61] A child from an indigenous community may spend a great deal of their time alongside family and community members doing different tasks and chores that benefit the community. After having seen the benefits of collaboration and work, and also having the opportunity to be included, the child will be intrinsically motivated to participate in similar tasks. In this example, because the adults in the community do not impose the tasks upon the children, the children therefore feel self-motivated and a desire to participate and learn through the task.[62] As a result of the community values that surround the child, their source of motivation may vary from a different community with different values.

In more Westernized communities, where segregation between adults and children participating in work related task is a common practice. As a result of this, these adolescents demonstrate less internalized motivation to do things within their environment than their parents. However, when the motivation to participate in activities is a prominent belief within the family, the adolescents autonomy is significantly higher. This therefore demonstrating that when collaboration and non-segregative tasks are norms within a child's upbringing, their internal motivation to participate in community tasks increases.[63] When given opportunities to work collaboratively with adults on shared tasks during childhood, children will therefore become more intrinsically motivated through adulthood.[64]

Social motivation is tied to one's activity in a group. It cannot form from a single mind alone. For example, bowling alone is naught but the dull act of throwing a ball into pins, and so people are much less likely to smile during the activity alone, even upon getting a strike because their satisfaction or dissatisfaction does not need to be communicated, and so it is internalized. However, when with a group, people are more inclined to smile regardless of their results because it acts as a positive communication that is beneficial for pleasurable interaction and teamwork.[60] Thus the act of bowling becomes a social activity as opposed to a dull action because it becomes an exercise in interaction, competition, team building, and sportsmanship. It is because of this phenomenon that studies have shown that people are more intrigued in performing mundane activities so long as there is company because it provides the opportunity to interact in one way or another, be it for bonding, amusement, collaboration, or alternative perspectives.[60] Examples of activities that may one may not be motivated to do alone but could be done with others for social benefit are things such as throwing and catching a baseball with a friend, making funny faces with children, building a treehouse, and performing a debate.

Push and pull[edit]

Push[edit]

Push motivations are those where people push themselves towards their goals or to achieve something, such as the desire for escape, rest and relaxation, prestige, health and fitness, adventure, and social interaction.[65]

However, with push motivation it's also easy to get discouraged when there are obstacles present in the path of achievement. Push motivation acts as a willpower and people's willpower is only as strong as the desire behind the willpower.[66]

Additionally, a study has been conducted on social networking and its push and pull effects. One thing that is mentioned is "Regret and dissatisfaction correspond to push factors because regret and dissatisfaction are the negative factors that compel users to leave their current service provider."[67] So from reading this, we now know that Push motivations can also be a negative force. In this case, that negative force is regret and dissatisfaction.

Pull[edit]

Pull motivation is the opposite of push. It is a type of motivation that is much stronger. "Some of the factors are those that emerge as a result of the attractiveness of a destination as it is perceived by those with the propensity to travel. They include both tangible resources, such as beaches, recreation facilities, and cultural attractions, and traveler's perceptions and expectation, such as novelty, benefit expectation, and marketing image."[65] Pull motivation can be seen as the desire to achieve a goal so badly that it seems that the goal is pulling us toward it. That is why pull motivation is stronger than push motivation. It is easier to be drawn to something rather than to push yourself for something you desire. It can also be an alternative force when compared to negative force. From the same study as previously mentioned, "Regret and dissatisfaction with an existing SNS service provider may trigger a heightened interest toward switching service providers, but such a motive will likely translate into reality in the presence of a good alternative. Therefore, alternative attractiveness can moderate the effects of regret and dissatisfaction with switching intention"[67] And so, pull motivation can be an attracting desire when negative influences come into the picture.

Self-control[edit]

Main articles: Self-control and Inhibitory control

The self-control aspect of motivation is increasingly considered to be a subset of emotional intelligence;[68] it is suggested that although a person may be classed as highly intelligent (as measured by many traditional intelligence tests), they may remain unmotivated to pursue intellectual endeavours. Vroom's "expectancy theory" provides an account of when people may decide to exert self-control in pursuit of a particular goal.

Drives[edit]

Main article: Drive theory

A drive or desire can be described as a deficiency or need that activates behavior that is aimed at a goal or an incentive.[69] These drives are thought to originate within the individual and may not require external stimuli to encourage the behavior. Basic drives could be sparked by deficiencies such as hunger, which motivates a person to seek food whereas more subtle drives might be the desire for praise and approval, which motivates a person to behave in a manner pleasing to others.

Another basic drive is the sexual drive which like food motivates us because it is essential to our survival.[70] The desire for sex is wired deep into the brain of all human beings as glands secrete hormones that travel through the blood to the brain and stimulates the onset of sexual desire.[70] The hormone involved in the initial onset of sexual desire is called Dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA).[70] The hormonal basis of both men and women's sex drives is testosterone.[70][need quotation to verify] Men naturally have more testosterone than women do and so are more likely than women to think about sex.[70][need quotation to verify]

Drive-reduction theory[edit]

Clark Hull was the behaviorist who developed the drive-reduction theory of motivation.

Drive theory grows out of the concept that people have certain biological drives, such as hunger and thirst. As time passes the strength of the drive increases if it is not satisfied (in this case by eating). Upon satisfying a drive the drive's strength is reduced. Created by Clark Hull and further developed by Kenneth Spence, the theory became well known in the 1940s and 1950s. Many of the motivational theories that arose during the 1950s and 1960s were either based on Hull's original theory or were focused on providing alternatives to the drive-reduction theory, including Abraham Maslow's hierarchy of needs, which emerged as an alternative to Hull's approach.[71]

Drive theory has some intuitive validity. For instance when preparing food, the drive model appears to be compatible with sensations of rising hunger as the food is prepared, and, after the food has been consumed, a decrease in subjective hunger.[72] There are several problems, however, that leave the validity of drive reduction open for debate[which?].

Cognitive dissonance theory[edit]

Main article: Cognitive dissonance

Suggested by Leon Festinger, cognitive dissonance occurs when an individual experiences some degree of discomfort resulting from an inconsistency between two cognitions: their views on the world around them, and their own personal feelings and actions.[citation needed] For example, a consumer may seek to reassure themselves regarding a purchase, feeling that another decision may have been preferable. Their feeling that another purchase would have been preferable is inconsistent with their action of purchasing the item. The difference between their feelings and beliefs causes dissonance, so they seek to reassure themselves.

While not a theory of motivation, per se, the theory of cognitive dissonance proposes that people have a motivational drive to reduce dissonance. The cognitive miser perspective makes people want to justify things in a simple way in order to reduce the effort they put into cognition. They do this by changing their attitudes, beliefs, or actions, rather than facing the inconsistencies, because dissonance is a mental strain. Dissonance is also reduced by justifying, blaming, and denying. It is one of the most influential and extensively studied theories in social psychology.

Temporal motivation theory[edit]

Main article: Temporal motivation theory

A recent approach in developing a broad, integrative theory of motivation is temporal motivation theory.[73] Introduced in a 2006 Academy of Management Review article,[74] it synthesizes into a single formulation the primary aspects of several other major motivational theories, including Incentive Theory, Drive Theory, Need Theory, Self-Efficacy and Goal Setting. It simplifies the field of motivation and allows findings from one theory to be translated into terms of another. Another journal article that helped to develop the Temporal Motivation Theory, "The Nature of Procrastination,[75] " received American Psychological Association's George A. Miller award for outstanding contribution to general science.

{\displaystyle \mathrm {Motivation} ={\frac {\mbox{Expectancy × Value}}{\mbox{1 + Impulsiveness × Delay}}}}

where Motivation is the desire for a particular outcome, Expectancy or self-efficacy is the probability of success, Value is the reward associated with the outcome, Impulsiveness is the individual's sensitivity to delay and Delay is the time to realization.[75]

Achievement motivation[edit]

Main article: Achievement motivation

Achievement motivation is an integrative perspective based on the premise that performance motivation results from the way broad components of personality are directed towards performance. As a result, it includes a range of dimensions that are relevant to success at work but which are not conventionally regarded as being part of performance motivation. The emphasis on performance seeks to integrate formerly separate approaches as need for achievement[76] with, for example, social motives like dominance. Personality is intimately tied to performance and achievement motivation, including such characteristics as tolerance for risk, fear of failure, and others.[77][78]

Achievement motivation can be measured by The Achievement Motivation Inventory, which is based on this theory and assesses three factors (in 17 separated scales) relevant to vocational and professional success. This motivation has repeatedly been linked with adaptive motivational patterns, including working hard, a willingness to pick learning tasks with much difficulty, and attributing success to effort.[79]

Achievement motivation was studied intensively by David C. McClelland, John W. Atkinson and their colleagues since the early 1950s.[80] This type of motivation is a drive that is developed from an emotional state. One may feel the drive to achieve by striving for success and avoiding failure. In achievement motivation, one would hope that they excel in what they do and not think much about the failures or the negatives.[81] Their research showed that business managers who were successful demonstrated a high need to achieve no matter the culture. There are three major characteristics of people who have a great need to achieve according to McClelland's research.

  1. They would prefer a work environment in which they are able to assume responsibility for solving problems.
  2. They would take calculated risk and establish moderate, attainable goals.
  3. They want to hear continuous recognition, as well as feedback, in order for them to know how well they are doing.[82]

Cognitive theories[edit]

Cognitive theories define motivation in terms of how people think about situations. Cognitive theories of motivation include goal-setting theory and expectancy theory.

Goal-setting theory[edit]

Main article: Goal-setting theory

Goal-setting theory is based on the idea that individuals have a drive to reach a clearly defined end state. Often, this end state is a reward in itself. A goal's efficiency is affected by three features: proximity, difficulty and specificity. One common goal setting methodology incorporates the SMART criteria, in which goals are: specific, measurable, attainable/achievable, relevant, and time-bound. Time management is an important aspect, when regarding time as a contributing factor to goal achievement. Having too much time allows for distraction and procrastination, which also serves as a distraction to the subject by steering their attention away from the original goal. An ideal goal should present a situation where the time between the beginning of effort and the end state is close.[83] With an overly restricting time restraint, the subject could potentially feel overwhelmed, which could deter the subject from achieving the goal because the amount of time provided is not sufficient or rational.[84] This explains why some children are more motivated to learn how to ride a bike than to master algebra. A goal should be moderate, not too hard or too easy to complete.[84]

Most people are not optimally motivated, as many want a challenge (which assumes some kind of insecurity of success). At the same time people want to feel that there is a substantial probability that they will succeed. The goal should be objectively defined and understandable for the individual.[83] Similarly to Maslow's Hierarchy of Needs, a larger end goal is easier to achieve if the subject has smaller, more attainable yet still challenging goals to achieve first in order to advance over a period of time.[84] A classic example of a poorly specified goal is trying to motivate oneself to run a marathon when s/he has not had proper training. A smaller, more attainable goal is to first motivate oneself to take the stairs instead of an elevator or to replace a stagnant activity, like watching television, with a mobile one, like spending time walking and eventually working up to a jog.[note 1]

Expectancy theory[edit]

Main article: Expectancy theory

Expectancy theory was proposed by Victor H. Vroom in 1964. Expectancy theory explains the behavior process in which an individual selects a behavior option over another, and why/how this decision is made in relation to their goal.

There's also an equation for this theory which goes as follows:

{\displaystyle {\text{M}}={\text{E}}\times {\text{I}}\times {\text{V}}} or
{\displaystyle {\text{Motivation}}={\text{Expectancy}}\times {\text{Instrumentality}}\times {\text{Valence}}}[85]
  • M (Motivation) is the amount an individual will be motivated by the condition or environment they placed themselves in. Which is based from the following hence the equation.
  • E (Expectancy) is the person's perception that effort will result in performance. In other words, it's the person assessment of how well and what kind of effort will relate in better performance.
  • I (Instrumentality) is the person's perception that performance will be rewarded or punished.
  • V (Valence) is the perceived amount of the reward or punishment that will result from the performance."[85]

Procrastination[edit]

Main article: Procrastination

Procrastination is the act to voluntarily postpone or delay an intended course of action despite anticipating that you will be worse off because of that delay.[53] While procrastination was once seen as a harmless habit, recent studies indicate otherwise. In a 1997 study conducted by Dianne Tice and William James Fellow Roy Baumeister at Case Western University, college students were given ratings on an established scale of procrastination, and tracked their academic performance, stress, and health throughout the semester. While procrastinators experienced some initial benefit in the form of lower stress levels (presumably by putting off their work at first), they ultimately earned lower grades and reported higher levels of stress and illness.[86]

Procrastination can be seen as a defense mechanism.[87] Because it is less demanding to simply avoid a task instead of dealing with the possibility of failure, procrastinators choose the short-term gratification of delaying a task over the long-term uncertainty of undertaking it. Procrastination can also be a justification for when the user ultimately has no choice but to undertake a task and performs below their standard. For example, a term paper could be seem as a daunting task. If the user puts it off until the night before, they can justify their poor score by telling themselves that they would have done better with more time. This kind of justification is extremely harmful and only helps to perpetuate the cycle of procrastination.[88]

Over the years, scientists have determined that not all procrastination is the same. The first type are chronic procrastinators whom exhibit a combination of qualities from the other, more specialized types of procrastinators. "Arousal" types are usually self-proclaimed "pressure performers" and relish the exhilaration of completing tasks close to the deadline. "Avoider" types procrastinate to avoid the outcome of whatever task they are pushing back - whether it be a potential failure or success. "Avoider" types are usually very self-conscious and care deeply about other people's opinions. Lastly, "Decisional" procrastinators avoid making decisions in order to protect themselves from the responsibility that follows the outcome of events.[89][failed verification]

Models of behavior change[edit]

See also: Cognitive control

Social-cognitive models of behavior change include the constructs of motivation and volition. Motivation is seen as a process that leads to the forming of behavioral intentions. Volition is seen as a process that leads from intention to actual behavior. In other words, motivation and volition refer to goal setting and goal pursuit, respectively. Both processes require self-regulatory efforts. Several self-regulatory constructs are needed to operate in orchestration to attain goals. An example of such a motivational and volitional construct is perceived self-efficacy. Self-efficacy is supposed to facilitate the forming of behavioral intentions, the development of action plans, and the initiation of action. It can support the translation of intentions into action.

John W. Atkinson, David Birch and their colleagues developed the theory of "Dynamics of Action" to mathematically model change in behavior as a consequence of the interaction of motivation and associated tendencies toward specific actions.[90][91] The theory posits that change in behavior occurs when the tendency for a new, unexpressed behavior becomes dominant over the tendency currently motivating action. In the theory, the strength of tendencies rises and falls as a consequence of internal and external stimuli (sources of instigation), inhibitory factors, and consummatory in factors such as performing an action. In this theory, there are three causes responsible for behavior and change in behavior:

  1. Instigation (Ts) – increases tendency when an activity has intrinsic ability to satisfy;
  2. Inhibition (Taf) – decreases tendency when there are obstacles to performing an activity; and
  3. Consummation – decreases a tendency as it is performed.[92][93]

Thematic apperception test[edit]

Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) was developed by American psychologists Henry A. Murray and Christina D. Morgan at Harvard during the early 1930s. Their underlying goal was to test and discover the dynamics of personality such as internal conflict, dominant drives, and motives. Testing is derived of asking the individual to tell a story, given 31 pictures that they must choose ten to describe. To complete the assessment, each story created by the test subject must be carefully recorded and monitored to uncover underlying needs and patterns of reactions each subject perceives. After evaluation, two common methods of research, Defense Mechanisms Manual (DMM) and Social Cognition and Object Relations (SCOR), are used to score each test subject on different dimensions of object and relational identification. From this, the underlying dynamics of each specific personality and specific motives and drives can be determined.

Attribution theory[edit]

Main article: Attribution (psychology)

Attribution theory describes individual's motivation to formulate explanatory attributions ("reasons") for events they experience, and how these beliefs affect their emotions and motivations.[94] Attributions are predicted to alter behavior, for instance attributing failure on a test to a lack of study might generate emotions of shame and motivate harder study. Important researchers include Fritz Heider and Bernard Weiner. Weiner's theory differentiates intrapersonal and interpersonal perspectives. Intrapersonal includes self-directed thoughts and emotions that are attributed to the self. The interpersonal perspective includes beliefs about the responsibility of others and emotions directed at other people, for instance attributing blame to another individual.[95]

Approach versus avoidance[edit]

Main article: Motivational salience

Approach motivation (i.e., incentive salience) can be defined as when a certain behavior or reaction to a situation/environment is rewarded or results in a positive or desirable outcome. In contrast, avoidance motivation (i.e., aversive salience) can be defined as when a certain behavior or reaction to a situation/environment is punished or results in a negative or undesirable outcome.[96][97] Research suggests that, all else being equal, avoidance motivations tend to be more powerful than approach motivations. Because people expect losses to have more powerful emotional consequences than equal-size gains, they will take more risks to avoid a loss than to achieve a gain.[96]

Conditioned taste aversion.

“A strong dislike (nausea reaction) for food because of prior Association with of that food with nausea or upset stomach.” [13]

Conditioned taste aversion is the only type of conditioning that only needs one exposure. It does not need to be the specific food or drinks that cause the taste. Conditioned taste aversion can also be attributed to extenuating circumstances. An example of this can be eating a rotten apple. Eating the apple then immediately throwing up. Now it is hard to even near an apple without feeling sick. Conditioned taste aversion can also come about by the mere associations of two stimuli. Eating a peanut butter and jelly sandwich, but also have the flu. Eating the sandwich makes one feel nauseous, so one throws up, now one cannot smell peanut butter without feeling queasy. Though eating the sandwich does not cause one to through up, they are still linked.[13]

Unconscious Motivation[edit]

In his book A General Introduction to Psychoanalysis, Sigmund Freud explained his theory on the conscious-unconscious distinction.[98] To explain this relationship, he used a two-room metaphor. The smaller of the two rooms is filled with a person's preconscious, which is the thoughts, emotions, and memories that are available to a person's consciousness. This room also houses a person's consciousness, which is the part of the preconscious that is the focus at that given time. Connected to the small room is a much larger room that houses a person's unconscious. This part of the mind is unavailable to a person's consciousness and consists of impulses and repressed thoughts. The door between these two rooms acts as the person's mental censor. Its job is to keep anxiety inducing thoughts and socially unacceptable behaviors or desires out of the preconscious. Freud describes the event of a thought or impulse being denied at the door as repression, one of the many defense mechanisms. This process is supposed to protect the individual from any embarrassment that could come from acting on these impulses or thoughts that exist in the unconscious.

In terms of motivation, Freud argues that unconscious instinctual impulses can still have great influence on behavior even though the person is not aware of the source.[99] When these instincts serve as a motive, the person is only aware of the goal of the motive, and not its actual source. He divides these instincts into sexual instincts, death instincts, and ego or self-preservation instincts. Sexual instincts are those that motivate humans to stay alive and ensure the continuation of the mankind. On the other hand, Freud also maintains that humans have an inherent drive for self-destruction, or the death instinct. Similar to the devil and angel that everyone has on there should, the sexual instinct and death instinct are constantly battling each other to both be satisfied. The death instinct can be closely related to Freud's other concept, the id, which is our need to experience pleasure immediately, regardless of the consequences. The last type of instinct that contributes to motivation is the ego or self-preservation instinct. This instinct is geared towards assuring that a person feels validated in whatever behavior or thought they have. The mental censor, or door between the unconscious and preconscious, helps satisfy this instinct. For example, one may be sexually attracted to a person, due to their sexual instinct, but the self-preservation instinct prevents them to act on this urge until that person finds that it is socially acceptable to do so. Quite similarly to his psychic theory that deals with the id, ego, and superego, Freud's theory of instincts highlights the interdependence of these three instincts. All three instincts serve as a checks and balances system to control what instincts are acted on and what behaviors are used to satisfy as many of them at once.

Priming[edit]

See also: Priming (psychology)

Priming is a phenomenon, often used as an experimental technique, whereby a specific stimulus sensitizes the subject to later presentation of a similar stimulus.[100]

“Priming refers to an increased sensitivity to certain stimuli, resulting from prior exposure to related visual or audio messages. When an individual is exposed to the word “cancer,” for example, and then offered the choice to smoke a cigarette, we expect that there is a greater probability that they will choose not to smoke as a result of the earlier exposure.”[101]

Priming can affect motivation, in the way that we can be motived to do things by an outside source.

Priming can be linked with the mere exposer theory. People tend to like things that they have been exposed to before. Mere exposer theory is used by advertising companies to get people to buy their products. An example of this is seeing a picture of the product on a sign and then buying that product later. If an individual is in a room with two strangers they are more likely to gravitate towards the person that they occasionally pass on the street, than the person that they have never seen before. An example of the use of mere exposure theory can be seen in product placements in movies and TV shows. We see a product that our is in our favorite movie, and we are more inclined to buy that product when we see it again.[102]

Priming can fit into these categories; Semantic Priming, Visual Priming, Response Priming, Perceptual and Conceptual Priming, Positive and Negative Priming, Associative and Context Priming, and Olfactory Priming. Visual and Semantic priming is the most used in motivation. Most priming is linked with emotion, the stronger the emotion, the stronger the connection between memory and the stimuli.[101]

Priming also has an effect on drug users. In this case, it can be defined at, the reinstatement or increase in drug craving by a small dose of the drug or by stimuli associated with the drug. If a former drug user is in a place where they formerly did drugs, then they are tempted to do that same thing again even if they have been clean for years.[13]

Conscious Motivation[edit]

Freud relied heavily upon the theories of unconscious motivation as explained above, but Allport (a researcher in 1967) looked heavily into the powers of conscious motivation and the effect it can have upon goals set for an individual. This is not to say that unconscious motivation should be ignored with this theory, but instead it focuses on the thought that if we are aware of our surroundings and our goals, we can then actively and consciously take steps towards them. [103]

He also believed that there are three hierarchical tiers of personality traits that affect this motivation:[103]

  1. Cardinal traits: Rare, but strongly determines a set behavior and can't be changed
  2. Central traits: Present around certain people, but can be hidden
  3. Secondary traits: Present in all people, but strongly reliant on context- can be altered as needed and would be the focus of a conscious motivation effort.

Mental Fatigue[edit]

Mental fatigue is being tired, exhausted, or not functioning effectively. Not wanting to proceed further with the current mental course of action, this in contrast with physical fatigue, because in most cases no physical activity is done.[104] This is best seen in the workplace or schools. A perfect example of mental fatigue is seen in college students just before finals approach. One will notice that students start eating more than they usually do and care less about interactions with friends and classmates. Mental fatigue arises when an individual becomes involved in a complex task but does no physical activity and is still worn out, the reason for this is because the brain uses about 20 percent of the human body's metabolic heart rate. The brain consumes about 10.8 calories every hour. Meaning that a typical human adult brain runs on about twelve watts of electricity or a fifth of the power need to power a standard light bulb.[105] These numbers represent an individual's brain working on routine tasks, things that are not challenging. One study suggests that when presented with a complex task, an individual would need to consume about two hundred more calories than if they were resting or relaxing. [105] The symptoms of mental fatigue can range from low motivation and loss of concentration to the more severe symptoms of headaches, dizziness, and impaired decision making and judgment. Mental fatigue can affect an individual's life by causing a lack of motivation, avoidance of friends and family members and changes in one's mood. To treat mental fatigue, one must figure out what is causing the fatigue. Once the cause of the stress has been identified the individual must determine what they can do about it. Most of the time mental fatigue can be fixed by a simple life change like being more organized or learning to say no.[106] According to the study: Mental fatigue caused by prolonged cognitive load associated with sympathetic hyperactivity, “there is evidence that decreased parasympathetic activity and increased relative sympathetic activity are associated with mental fatigue induced by prolonged cognitive load in healthy adults.[107]” this means that though no physical activity was done, the sympathetic nervous system was triggered. An individual who is experiencing mental fatigue will not feel relaxed but feel the physical symptoms of stress.

Learned Industriousness[edit]

Learned industriousness theory is the theory about an acquired ability to sustain the physical or mental effort. It can also be described as being persistent despite the building up subjective fatigue.[104] This is the ability to push through to the end for a greater or bigger reward. The more significant or more rewarding the incentive, the more the individual is willing to do to get to the end of a task.[108] This is one of the reasons that college students will go on to graduate school. The students may be worn out, but they are willing to go through more school for the reward of getting a higher paying job when they are out of school.

Practical applications[edit]

The control of motivation is only understood to a limited extent. There are many different approaches of motivation training, but many of these are considered pseudoscientific by critics[which?]. To understand how to control motivation it is first necessary to understand why many people lack motivation[original research?].

Like any theory, motivational theory makes predictions about what will work in practice. For instance McGregor's Theory Y makes the assumption that the average person not only accepts, but also seeks out responsibility, enjoys doing work and, therefore, is more satisfied when they have a wider range of work to do.[32] The practical implication is that, as a firm gives individuals’ greater responsibilities, they will feel a greater sense of satisfaction and, subsequently, more commitment to the organization. Likewise allocating more work is predicted to increase engagement. Additionally, Malone argues that the delegation of responsibility encourages motivation because employees have creative control over their work and increases productivity as many people can work collaboratively to solve a problem rather than just one manager tackling it alone.[109] Others have argued that participation in decision making boosts morale and commitment to the organization, subsequently increasing productivity.[110][111] Likewise, if teams and membership increase motivation (as reported in the classic Hawthorn Western Electric Company studies [33]) incorporating teams make provide incentives to work. In general, motivation theory is often applied to employee motivation.[112]

Applications in Business[edit]

Main articles: Work motivation, Job satisfaction, and Organizational behavior § Motivation

Within Maslow's hierarchy of needs (first proposed in 1943), at lower levels (such as physiological needs) money functions as a motivator; however, it tends to have a motivating effect on staff that lasts only for a short period (in accordance with Herzberg's two-factor model of motivation of 1959). At higher levels of the hierarchy, praise, respect, recognition, empowerment and a sense of belonging are far more powerful motivators than money, as both Abraham Maslow's theory of motivation and Douglas McGregor's theory X and theory Y (originating in the 1950s and pertaining to the theory of leadership) suggest.

According to Maslow, people are motivated by unsatisfied needs.[113] The lower-level needs (such as Physiological and Safety needs) must be satisfied before addressing higher-level needs. One can relate Maslow's Hierarchy of Needs theory with employee motivation. For example, if managers attempt to motivate their employees by satisfying their needs; according to Maslow, they should try to satisfy the lower-level needs before trying to satisfy the upper-level needs - otherwise the employees will not become motivated. Managers should also remember that not everyone will be satisfied by the same needs. A good manager will try to figure out which levels of needs are relevant to a given individual or employee.

Maslow places money at the lowest level of the hierarchy and postulates other needs as better motivators to staff. McGregor places money in his Theory X category and regards it as a poor motivator. Praise and recognition (placed in the Theory Y category) are considered stronger motivators than money.

The average workplace lies about midway between the extremes of high threat and high opportunity. Motivation by threat is a dead-end strategy, and naturally staff are more attracted to the opportunity side of the motivation curve than the threat side. Lawrence Steinmetz (1983) sees motivation as a powerful tool in the work environment that can lead to employees working at their most efficient levels of production.[114]

Nonetheless, Steinmetz also discusses three common character-types of subordinates: ascendant, indifferent, and ambivalent - who all react and interact uniquely, and must be treated, managed, and motivated accordingly. An effective leader must understand how to manage all characters, and more importantly the manager must utilize avenues that allow room for employees to work, grow, and find answers independently.[115][need quotation to verify]

A classic study at Vauxhall Motors' UK manufacturing plant challenged the assumptions of Maslow and Herzberg were by.[116] Goldthorpe et al. (1968) introduced the concept of orientation to work and distinguished three main orientations:

  • instrumental (with work seen as a means to an end)
  • bureaucratic (where work serves as a source of status, security and immediate reward)
  • solidaristic (which prioritizes group loyalty)

Other theories expanded and extended those of Maslow and Herzberg. These included the 1930s force-field analysis of Kurt Lewin, Edwin A. Locke's goal-setting theory (mid-1960s onwards) and Victor Vroom's expectancy theory of 1964. These tend to stress cultural differences and the fact that different factors tend to motivate individuals at different times.[117][need quotation to verify]

According to the system of scientific management developed by Frederick Winslow Taylor (1856-1915), pay alone determines a worker's motivation, and therefore management need not consider psychological or social aspects of work. In essence, scientific management bases human motivation wholly on extrinsic rewards and discards the idea of intrinsic rewards.

In contrast, David McClelland (1917-1998) believed that workers could not be motivated by the mere need for money—in fact, extrinsic motivation (e.g., money) could extinguish intrinsic motivation such as achievement motivation, though money could be used as an indicator of success for various motives, e.g., keeping score. In keeping with this view, his consulting firm, McBer & Company (1965-1989), had as its first motto "To make everyone productive, happy, and free". For McClelland, satisfaction lay in aligning peoples' lives with their fundamental motivations.

Elton Mayo (1880-1949) discovered the importance of the social contacts a worker has at the workplace and found that boredom and repetitiveness of tasks lead to reduced motivation. Mayo believed that workers could be motivated by acknowledging their social needs and making them feel important. As a result, employees were given freedom to make decisions on-the-job and greater attention was paid[by whom?] to informal work-groups.

Mayo named his model the Hawthorne effect.[citation needed] His model has been judged[by whom?] as placing undue reliance on social contacts within work situations for motivating employees.[118][need quotation to verify]

In 1981 William Ouchi introduced Theory Z, a hybrid management approach consisting of both Japanese and American philosophies and cultures.[119][need quotation to verify] Its Japanese segment is much like the clan culture where organizations focus on a standardized structure with heavy emphasis on socialization of its members. All underlying goals are consistent across the organization. Its American segment retains formality and authority amongst members and the organization. Ultimately, Theory Z promotes common structure and commitment to the organization, as well as constant improvement of work efficacy.

In Essentials of Organizational Behavior (2007), Robbins and Judge examine recognition programs as motivators, and identify five principles that contribute to the success of an employee-incentive program:[120]

  • recognition of employees' individual differences, and clear identification of behavior deemed worthy of recognition
  • allowing employees to participate
  • linking rewards to performance
  • rewarding of nominators
  • visibility of the recognition process

Modern organizations which adopt non-monetary employee motivation methods rather than tying it with tangible rewards. When the reward is about aimed at fulfilling employee contribution, participation and individual satisfaction, it boosts their morale.[121]

Job characteristics model[edit]

Main article: Job characteristics model

The Job characteristics Model (JCM), as designed by Hackman[122] and Oldham attempts to use job design to improve employee motivation. They suggest that any job can be described in terms of five key job characteristics:[123][124]

  1. Skill variety – the degree to which the job requires the use of different skills and talents
  2. Task identity – the degree to which the job has contributed to a clearly identifiable larger project
  3. Task significance – the degree to which the job affects the lives or work of other people
  4. Autonomy – the degree to which the worker has independence, freedom and discretion in carrying out the job
  5. Task feedback – the degree to which the worker is provided with clear, specific, detailed, actionable information about the effectiveness of his or her job performance

The JCM links the core job dimensions listed above to critical psychological states which results in desired personal and work outcomes. This forms the basis of this 'employee growth-need strength." The core dimensions listed above can be combined into a single predictive index, called the motivating potential score (MPS). The MPS can be calculated, using the core dimensions discussed above, as follows:

{\displaystyle {\text{MPS}}=}{\displaystyle {\text{Autonomy}}\,\times \,{\text{Feedback}}\,\times {\frac {\text{Skill Variety+Task Identity+Task Significance }}{\text{3}}}}

Jobs high in motivating potential must be high on both Autonomy and Feedback, and also must be high on at least one of the three factors that lead to experienced meaningfulness.[125] If a job has a high MPS, the job characteristics model predicts motivation, performance and job satisfaction will be positively affected and the likelihood of negative outcomes, such as absenteeism and turnover, will be reduced.[125]

Employee recognition programs[edit]

Employee recognition is not only about gifts and points. It's about changing the corporate culture in order to meet goals and initiatives and most importantly to connect employees to the company's core values and beliefs. Strategic employee recognition is seen as the most important program not only to improve employee retention and motivation but also to positively influence the financial situation.[126] The difference between the traditional approach (gifts and points) and strategic recognition is the ability to serve as a serious business influencer that can advance a company's strategic objectives in a measurable way. "The vast majority of companies want to be innovative, coming up with new products, business models and better ways of doing things. However, innovation is not so easy to achieve. A CEO cannot just order it, and so it will be. You have to carefully manage an organization so that, over time, innovations will emerge."[127]

Applications in Education[edit]

Motivation is of particular interest to educational psychologists because of the crucial role it plays in student learning. However, the specific kind of motivation that is studied in the specialized setting of education differs qualitatively from the more general forms of motivation studied by psychologists in other fields.

Motivation in education can have several effects on how students learn and how they behave towards subject matter.[128] It can:

  1. Direct behavior toward particular goals
  2. Lead to increased effort and energy
  3. Increase initiation of, and persistence in, activities
  4. Enhance cognitive processing
  5. Determine what consequences are reinforcing
  6. Lead to improved performance.

Because students are not always internally motivated, they sometimes need situated motivation, which is found in environmental conditions that the teacher creates.

If teachers decided to extrinsically reward productive student behaviors, they may find it difficult to extricate themselves from that path. Consequently, student dependency on extrinsic rewards represents one of the greatest detractors from their use in the classroom.[129]

The majority of new student orientation leaders at colleges and universities recognize that distinctive needs of students should be considered in regard to orientation information provided at the beginning of the higher education experience. Research done by Whyte in 1986 raised the awareness of counselors and educators in this regard. In 2007, the National Orientation Directors Association reprinted Cassandra B. Whyte's research report allowing readers to ascertain improvements made in addressing specific needs of students over a quarter of a century later to help with academic success.[130]

Generally, motivation is conceptualized as either intrinsic or extrinsic. Classically, these categories are regarded as distinct.[2] Today, these concepts are less likely to be used as distinct categories, but instead as two ideal types that define a continuum:[131]

  1. Intrinsic motivation occurs when people are internally motivated to do something because it either brings them pleasure, they think it is important, or they feel that what they are learning is significant. It has been shown that intrinsic motivation for education drops from grades 3-9 though the exact cause cannot be ascertained.[132] Also, in younger students it has been shown that contextualizing material that would otherwise be presented in an abstract manner increases the intrinsic motivation of these students.[133]
  2. Extrinsic motivation comes into play when a student is compelled to do something or act a certain way because of factors external to him or her (like money or good grades).

Cassandra B. Whyte researched and reported about the importance of locus of control and academic achievement. Students tending toward a more internal locus of control are more academically successful, thus encouraging curriculum and activity development with consideration of motivation theories.[134][135]

Academic motivation orientation may also be tied with one's ability to detect and process errors. Fisher, Nanayakkara, and Marshall conducted neuroscience research on children's motivation orientation, neurological indicators of error monitoring (the process of detecting an error), and academic achievement. Their research suggests that students with high intrinsic motivation attribute performance to personal control and that their error-monitoring system is more strongly engaged by performance errors. They also found that motivation orientation and academic achievement were related to the strength in which their error-monitoring system was engaged.[136]

Motivation has been found to be an important element in the concept of andragogy (what motivates the adult learner), and in treating Autism Spectrum Disorders, as in pivotal response treatment. Motivation has also been found critical in adolescents compliance to health suggestions, since "commitment requires belief in potentially negative and serious consequences of not acting".[137]

Doyle and Moeyn have noted that traditional methods tended to use anxiety as negative motivation (e.g. use of bad grades by teachers) as a method of getting students to work. However, they have found that progressive approaches with focus on positive motivation over punishment has produced greater effectiveness with learning, since anxiety interferes with performance of complex tasks.[138]

Symer et al. attempted to better define those in medical training programs who may have a ”surgical personality”. They evaluated a group of eight hundred and one first-year surgical interns to compare motivational traits amongst those who did and did not complete surgical training. There was no difference noted between the 80.5% who completed training when comparing their responses to the 19.5% who did not complete training using the validated Behavior Inhibitory System/Behavior Approach System. They concluded based on this that resident physician motivation is not associated with completion of a surgical training program.[139]

It may appear that the reason some students are more engaged and perform better in class activities relative to other students is because some are more motivated than others. However, current research suggests that motivation is "dynamic, context sensitive, and changeable."[140] Thus, students have the flexibility to alter their motivation for engaging in an activity or learning, even if they were not intrinsically motivated in the first place.[141] While having this type of flexibility is important, research reveals that a teacher's teaching style and the school environment may play a factor in student motivation.[142][140][143]

According to Sansone and Morgan, when students are already motivated to engage in an activity for their own personal pleasure and then a teacher provides the student with feedback, the type of feedback given can change the way that student views the activity and can even undermine their intrinsic motivation.[141][144] Maclellan also looked at the relationship between tutors and students and in particular, and the type of feedback the tutor would give to the student. Maclellan's results showed that praise or criticism directed towards the student generated a feeling of “fixed intelligence” while praise and criticism directed towards the effort and strategy used by the student generated a feeling of “malleable intelligence”.[140] In other words, feedback concerning effort and strategy leaves students knowing that there is room for growth. This is important because when students believe their intelligence is “fixed”, their mindset can prevent skill development because students will believe that they only have a “certain amount” of understanding on a particular subject matter and might not even try.Therefore, it's crucial that a teacher is aware of how the feedback they give to their students can both positively and negatively impact the student's engagement and motivation.[140][142]

In a correlational study, Katz and Shahar used a series of questionnaires and Likert-style scales and gave them to 100 teachers to see what makes a motivating teacher. Their results indicate that teachers who are intrinsically motivated to teach and believe that students should be taught in an autonomous style are the types of teachers that promote intrinsic motivation in the classroom.[142] Deci, Sheinman, and Nezlek also found that when teachers adapted to an autonomous teaching style, students were positively affected and became more intrinsically motivated to achieve in the classroom. However, while the students were quick to adapt to the new teaching style the impact was short-lived.[145] Thus, teachers are limited in the way they teach because they'll feel a pressure to act, teach, and provide feedback in a certain way from the school district, administration, and guardians.[145][143] Furthermore, even if students do have a teacher that promotes an autonomous teaching style, their overall school environment is also a factor because it can be extrinsically motivating. Examples of this would be posters around school promoting pizza parties for highest grade point average or longer recess times for the classroom that brings more canned food donations.

In conclusion, it is not a matter whether a student is motivated, unmotivated, or more motivated than other students- it's a matter of understanding what motivates students before providing a certain type of feedback. Furthermore, it is also important to note that despite the classroom environment and the teacher's teaching style, the overall school environment plays a role in students’ intrinsic motivation.

Indigenous education and learning[edit]

For many indigenous students (such as Native American children), motivation may be derived from social organization; an important factor educators should account for in addition to variations in sociolinguistics and cognition.[146] While poor academic performance among Native American students is often attributed to low levels of motivation, Top-down classroom organization is often found to be ineffective for children of many cultures who depend on a sense of community, purpose, and competence in order to engage.[147] Horizontally structured, community-based learning strategies often provide a more structurally supportive environment for motivating indigenous children, who tend to be driven by "social/affective emphasis, harmony, holistic perspectives, expressive creativity, and nonverbal communication."[148] This drive is also traceable to a cultural tradition of community-wide expectations of participation in the activities and goals of the greater group, rather than individualized aspirations of success or triumph.[149]

Also, in some indigenous communities, young children can often portray a sense of community-based motivation through their parent-like interactions with siblings.[150] Furthermore, it is commonplace for children to assist and demonstrate for their younger counterparts without being prompted by authority figures. Observation techniques and integration methods are demonstrated in such examples as weaving in Chiapas, Mexico, where it is commonplace for children to learn from "a more skilled other" within the community.[151] The child's real responsibility within the Mayan community can be seen in, for example, weaving apprenticeships; often, when the "more skilled other" is tasked with multiple obligations, an older child will step in and guide the learner.[151] Sibling guidance is supported from early youth, where learning through play encourages horizontally structured environments through alternative educational models such as "Intent Community Participation."[152] Research also suggests that formal Westernized schooling can actually reshape the traditionally collaborative nature of social life in indigenous communities.[153] This research is supported cross-culturally, with variations in motivation and learning often reported higher between indigenous groups and their national Westernized counterparts than between indigenous groups across international continental divides.[154]

Also, in some Indigenous communities in the Americas, motivation is a driving force for learning. Children are incorporated and welcomed to participate in daily activities and thus feel motivated to participate due to them seeking a sense of belonging in their families and communities.[155]

Children's participation is encouraged and their learning is supported by their community and family, furthering their motivation. Children are also trusted to be active contributors. Their active participation allows them to learn and gain skills that are valuable and useful in their communities.[156]

As children transition from early childhood to middle childhood, their motivation to participate changes. In both the Indigenous communities of Quechua people and Rioja in Peru, children often experience a transition in which they become more included into their family's and community's endeavors. This changes their position and role in their families to more responsible ones and leads to an increase in their eagerness to participate and belong. As children go through this transition, they often develop a sense of identity within their family and community.[157]

The transition from childhood to adolescence can be seen in the amount of work children partake in as this changes over time. For example, Yucatec Mayan children's play time decreases from childhood to adolescence and as the child gets older, is replaced for time spent working. In childhood the work is initiated by others whereas in adolescence it is self-initiated. The shift in initiation and the change in time spent working versus playing shows the children's motivation to participate in order to learn.[158]

This transition between childhood and adolescence increases motivation because children gain social responsibility within their families. In some Mexican communities of Indigenous-heritage, the contributions that children make within their community is essential to being social beings, establishes their developing roles, and also helps with developing their relationship with their family and community.[159]

As children gain more roles and responsibilities within their families, their eagerness to participate also increases. For example, Young Mayan children of San Pedro, Guatemala learn to work in the fields and family run businesses because they are motivated to contribute to their family. Many San Pedro women learned to weave by watching their mothers sew when they were children, sometimes earning their own wool through doing small tasks such as watching young children of busy mothers. Eager to learn and contribute, these young girls helped other members of their community in order to help their mothers with their weaving businesses or through other tasks such as helping carry water while young boys helped with tasks such as carrying firewood alongside their fathers.[160]

Children's motivation to learn is not solely influenced on their desire to belong but also their eagerness to see their community succeed. Children from Navajo communities were shown to have higher levels of social concern than Anglo American children in their schools. By having high levels of social concern the indigenous children are showing concern for not only their learning but also their peers', which serves as an example of their instilled sense of responsibility for their community. They wish to succeed as a united group rather than just themselves.[161]

In order to be knowledgeable contributors, children must be aware of their surroundings and community's goals. Children's learning in Indigenous-heritage communities is mainly based upon observing and helping out others in their community. Through this type of participation within their community, they gain purpose and motivation for the activity that they are doing within their community and become active participants because they know they are doing it for their community.[162]

Self-determination in education[edit]

Self-determination is the ability to make choices and exercise a high degree of control, such as what the student does and how they do it (Deci et al., 1991; Reeve, Hamm, & Nix, 2003; Ryan & Deci, 2002). Self-determination can be supported by providing opportunities for students to be challenged, such as leadership opportunities, providing appropriate feedback and fostering, establishing and maintaining good relationships between teachers and students. These strategies can increase students' interest, competence, creativity and desire to be challenged and ensure that students are intrinsically motivated to study. On the other hand, students who lack self-determination are more likely to feel their success is out of their control. Such students lose motivation to study, which causes a state of "learned helplessness". Students who feel helpless readily believe they will fail and therefore cease to try. Over time, a vicious circle of low achievement develops.

Physical activity in education[edit]

Physical activity is body movement that works your muscles and requires more energy than resting. According to a blog by the American Intercontinental University, college students should make time for exercise to maintain and increase motivation. AIU states that regular exercise has impeccable effects on the brain. With consistent running routines, there are more complex connections between neurons, meaning the brain is able to access its brain cells more flexibly. By performing well physically, motivation will be present in education because of how well the brain is performing. After exercising, the brain can have more desire to obtain knowledge and better retain the information. In addition, exercise can relieve stress. Exercising can ease anxiety and relieve negative effects of stress on the body. Without stress factors, individuals can perform better and more efficiently, since their minds will have a more positive outlook. This positive mood will help keep students motivated and more open and willing to succeed academically. Lastly, exercise increases focus and concentration that could also help students maintain their motivation and focus on their studies. AIU claims that exercise may have improved the students' ability to participate and retain information during the class after they had exercised. Being able to retain information and being willing to participate keeps students motivated and performing well academically.[163]

Applications in Game Design[edit]

Motivational models are central to game design, because without motivation, a player will not be interested in progressing further within a game.[164] Several models for gameplay motivations have been proposed, including Richard Bartle's. Jon Radoff has proposed a four-quadrant model of gameplay motivation that includes cooperation, competition, immersion and achievement.[165] The motivational structure of games is central to the gamification trend, which seeks to apply game-based motivation to business applications.[166] In the end, game designers must know the needs and desires of their customers for their companies to flourish.

There have been various studies on the connection between motivation and games. One particular study was on Taiwanese adolescents and their drive of addiction to games. Two studies by the same people were conducted. The first study revealed that addicted players showed higher intrinsic than extrinsic motivation and more intrinsic motivation than the non-addicted players.[167] It can then be said that addicted players, according to the studies findings, are more internally motivated to play games. They enjoy the reward of playing. There are studies that also show that motivation gives these players more to look for in the future such as long-lasting experience that they may keep later on in life.[168]

See also[edit]

Notes[edit]

  1. ^ All goals are subject to the individual's skills and abilities.

References[edit]

  1. ^ Locke, E. A. & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
  2. ^ Jump up to:a b c d e f g h i Ryan, Richard M.; Deci, Edward L. (2000). "Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivations: Classic Definitions and New Directions". Contemporary Educational Psychology. 25 (1): 54–67. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.318.808. doi:10.1006/ceps.1999.1020. PMID 10620381.
  3. ^ Jump up to:a b c d e f g h i Ryan, Richard M.; Deci, Edward L. (2000). "Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being". American Psychologist. 55(1): 68–78. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.529.4370. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68.
  4. ^ Latham, Gary P. (2012). Work Motivation: History, Theory, Research, and Practice. Los Angeles: Sage.
  5. ^ Pinder, C. C. (1998). Work motivation in organizational behavior. Upper Saddle River NJ: Practice Hall.
  6. ^ Locke, (Ed..), Edwin (2009). Attain emotional control by understanding what emotions are. In Handbook of principles of organizational behavior. New York: Wiley.
  7. ^ Robbins, Trevor W.; Everitt, Barry J. (1996). "Neurobehavioural mechanisms of reward and motivation". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 6 (2): 228–236. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(96)80077-8.
  8. ^ Berridge, Kent C; Kringelbach, Morten L (2013). "Neuroscience of affect: brain mechanisms of pleasure and displeasure". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 23 (3): 294–303. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.017. PMC 3644539. PMID 23375169.
  9. ^ Salamone, John D.; Correa, Mercè (8 November 2012). "The mysterious motivational functions of mesolimbic dopamine". Neuron. 76 (3): 470–485. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.021. ISSN 0896-6273. PMC 4450094. PMID 23141060.
  10. ^ Jump up to:a b c d Rueda, Richard; Moll, Luis C. (1994). "Chapter 7: A Sociocultural Perspective on Motivation". In O'Neill, Jr., Harold F.; Drillings, Michael (eds.). Motivation: Theory and Research. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. ISBN 978-0-8058-1286-2.
  11. ^ Pardee, R. L. (1990). The basic concept behind the hierarchy system is that it's like a food pyramid. Everybody starts at the bottom of the pyramid and are motivated to satisfy each level in the ascending order to work our way to the top of the pyramid, and those levels (needs) are categorized into two main groups with five difference sections which are explained below. Motivation Theories of Maslow, Herzberg, McGregor & McClelland. A Literature Review of Selected Theories Dealing with Job Satisfaction and Motivation.
  12. ^ "The Content Theories of Motivation".
  13. ^ Jump up to:a b c d e f g h Deckers, Lambert (2018). Motivation Biological, Psychological, and Environmental (5th ed.). 711 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10017: Routledge. pp. 30–38, 71–75. ISBN 9781138036321.
  14. ^ Chandler, H (1975). "Hedonism". American Philosophical Quarterly. 12 (3): 223–233. JSTOR 20009578.
  15. ^ Murray, S (2018). "Heterosexual Men's Sexual Desire: Supported by, or Deviating from, Traditional Masculinity Norms and Sexual Scripts?". Sex Roles. 78 (1): 130–141. doi:10.1007/s11199-017-0766-7.
  16. ^ Gonzaga, G.; Turner, R.; Keltner, D.; Campos, B.; Altemus, M.; Davidson, Richard J.; Scherer, Klaus R. (2006). "Romantic Love and Sexual Desire in Close Relationships". Emotion. 6 (2): 163–179. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.163.
  17. ^ Gonzaga, G. C.; Keltner, D. A.; Londahl, E. D.; Smith, M. (2001). "Love and the commitment problem in romantic relations and friendship". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81(2): 247–262. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.247.
  18. ^ Hosken, David J.; House, Clarissa M. "Sexual Selection". Current Biology. 21 (2): R62–R65. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.053.
  19. ^ Pardee, R. L. (1990). Motivation Theories of Maslow, Herzberg, McGregor & McClelland. A Literature Review of Selected Theories Dealing with Job Satisfaction and Motivation.
  20. ^ Schneider, Benjamin; Alderfer, Clayton P. (1973). "Three Studies of Measures of Need Satisfaction in Organizations". Administrative Science Quarterly. 18 (4): 489–505. doi:10.2307/2392201. JSTOR 2392201.
  21. ^ Deci, Edward L. (1971). "Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 18 (1): 105–115. doi:10.1037/H0030644.
  22. ^ Harter, S (1978). "Effectance motivation reconsidered: Toward a developmental model". Human Development. 1: 661–669.
  23. ^ White, R. W. (1963). Ego and reality in psychoanalytic theory. New York: International Universities Press.
  24. ^ Baumeister, R.; Leary, M. R. (1995). "The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation". Psychological Bulletin. 117 (3): 497–529. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497. PMID 7777651.
  25. ^ deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation. New York: Academic Press.
  26. ^ Deci, Edward L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum. ISBN 978-1-4613-4448-3.
  27. ^ Deci, Edward L.; Ryan, Richard M. (October 2000). "The "What" and "Why" of Goal Pursuits: Human Needs and the Self-Determination of Behavior" (PDF). Psychological Inquiry. 11(4): 227–268. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1104_01.
  28. ^ Deci, Edward L.; Vansteenkiste, Maarten (2004). "Self-determination theory and basic need satisfaction: Understanding human development in positive psychology". Ricerche di Psichologia. 27: 17–34.
  29. ^ E.L. Deci; R. M. Ryan (2008). "Self-determination theory: A macrotheory of human motivation, development, and health". Canadian Psychology. 49 (3): 182–185. doi:10.1037/a0012801.
  30. ^ "New Theory of Motivation Lists 16 Basic Desires That Guide Us". Research News. Ohio State. 2000-06-28. Retrieved 2012-06-02.
  31. ^ Jump up to:a b Dobbin, Frank. “From Incentives to Teamwork: Rational and Natural Management Systems.” Lecture. Harvard University. Cambridge, Massachusetts. 1 October 2012.
  32. ^ Jump up to:a b c d e McGregor, D., 1960. The Human Side of Enterprise, New York, McGraw-Hill.
  33. ^ Jump up to:a b c d Elton Mayo, 1984 [1949]. “Hawthorne and the Western Electric Company.” Pp. 279-292 in Organization Theory: Selected Readings. Second Edition. Edited by D.S. Pugh. New York: Penguin.
  34. ^ Jump up to:a b c d e Roethlisberger, F. J., et al. Management and the Worker; an Account of a Research Program Conducted by the Western Electric Company, Hawthorne Works, Chicago,. Cambridge, Mass.,: Harvard university press, 1939. Print.
  35. ^ Walton, Cohen, Gregory, Geoffrey (2011). Sharing Motivation. New York: Psychology Press. pp. 82–83.
  36. ^ "Motivation". panoramaconsulting.co.ke. Archived from the original on 3 January 2015. Retrieved 8 December 2014.
  37. ^ Jump up to:a b Deci, Koestner, Ryan, Edward, Richard, Richard (1999). "A Meta-Analytic Review of Experiments Examining the Effects of Extrinsic Rewards on Intrinsic Motivation". Psychological Bulletin. 125 (6): 627–668. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.627. PMID 10589297.
  38. ^ Jump up to:a b Ryan, Richard; Edward L. Deci (2000). "Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivations: Classic Definitions and New Directions". Contemporary Educational Psychology. 25 (1): 54–67. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.318.808. doi:10.1006/ceps.1999.1020. PMID 10620381.
  39. ^ Deci, Edward L.; Ryan, Richard M. (2013-06-29). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York. ISBN 9781489922717. OCLC 861705534.
  40. ^ Wigfield, A.; Guthrie, J. T.; Tonks, S.; Perencevich, K. C. (2004). "Children's motivation for reading: Domain specificity and instructional influences". Journal of Educational Research. 97 (6): 299–309. doi:10.3200/joer.97.6.299-310.
  41. ^ N Root III, George. "Examples of Intrinsic Workplace Motivation". Chron. Retrieved 27 November 2014.
  42. ^ Benjamin Lowry, Paul; Gaskin, James; Twyman, Nathan W.; Hammer, Bryan; Roberts, Tom L. (2013). "Taking 'fun and games' seriously: Proposing the hedonic-motivation system adoption model (HMSAM)". Journal of the Association for Information Systems. 14 (11): 617–671. doi:10.17705/1jais.00347. SSRN 2177442.
  43. ^ Parker, Christopher J.; Wang, Huchen (2016). "Examining hedonic and utilitarian motivations for m-commerce fashion retail app engagement". Journal of Fashion Marketing and Management. 20 (4): 487–506. doi:10.1108/JFMM-02-2016-0015.
  44. ^ StoryStudio, . "One type of therapy is addressing autism, dementia, and so much more." San Francisco Gate, California Applied Behavior Analysis , 4 Aug. 2017, blog.sfgate.com/storystudio/2017/08/04/one-type-of-therapy-is-tackling-autism-dementia-and-so-much-more/. Accessed 15 Nov. 2017.
  45. ^ Jump up to:a b Deci,Ryan, Edward,Richard (2000). "Self-Determination Theory and the Facilitation of Intrinsic Motivation, Social Development, and Well-Being". American Psychologist: 70–71.
  46. ^ Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York, NY: Plenum
  47. ^ Deci & Ryan. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist.
  48. ^ "Motivating Students". 2010-06-11. Retrieved 4 March 2015.
  49. ^ Dewani, Vijay (2013-01-12). "Motivation". slideshare. Retrieved 22 March 2013.
  50. ^ Lepper, Mark R.; Greene, David; Nisbet, Richard (1973). "Undermining Children's Intrinsic Interest with Extrinsic Reward; A Test of 'Overjustification' Hypothesis". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 28: 129–37. doi:10.1037/h0035519.
  51. ^ Marinak, Barbara A.; Gambrell, Linda B. (2008). "Intrinsic Motivation and Rewards: What Sustains Young Children's Engagement with Text?". Literacy Research and Instruction. 47: 9–26. doi:10.1080/19388070701749546.
  52. ^ Wilson, T. D.; Lassiter, G. D. (1982). "Increasing intrinsic interest with superfluous extrinsic constraints". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 42 (5): 811–819. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.42.5.811.
  53. ^ Jump up to:a b Lambert., Deckers (2014). Motivation : biological, psychological, environmental. Pearson. ISBN 9781292027999. OCLC 1040982893.
  54. ^ Jump up to:a b c "Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi: All About Flow & Positive Psychology". 2016-12-16. Retrieved 2018-11-10.
  55. ^ "Can't Stop Worrying? Try Tetris To Ease Your Mind". NPR.org. Retrieved 2018-11-10.
  56. ^ "Flow and Happiness". Psychology Today. Retrieved 2018-11-10.
  57. ^ Jump up to:a b c d e f g h Cooper, John O (2007). Applied Behavior Analysis. Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA: Pearson Education. ISBN 978-0-13-129327-4.
  58. ^ Jump up to:a b c d e Donahoe, J.W. (2004). Learning and Complex Behavior. Richmond, MA, USA: Ledgetop Publishing. ISBN 978-0-9762371-0-5.
  59. ^ Rogoff, Barbara (2009). "Side by Side: Learning by Observing and Pitching In". Journal of the Society for Psychological Anthropology: 102–138.
  60. ^ Jump up to:a b c Walton, Gregory; Cohen, Geoffrey (2011). "Sharing Motivation". Social Motivation: 79–101.
  61. ^ Bolin, Inge (January 2006). Growing Up in a Culture of Respect.
  62. ^ Murray, Bowen, Segura, Verdugo, Marjorie, Sofia, Nicole, Marisol (2015). "Apprehending Volition in Early Socialization: Raising "Little Persons" among Rural Mapuche Families". Ethos. 43 (4): 376–401. doi:10.1111/etho.12094.
  63. ^ Gronhoj,Rhogersen, Alice, John (2017). "Why young people do things for the environment: the role of parenting for adolescents motivation to engage in pro-environmental behavior". Journal of Environmental Psychology.
  64. ^ Bolin, Inge (2006). Growing Up in a Culture of Respect. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.
  65. ^ Jump up to:a b Uysal, Muzaffer (1994). "Testing the push and pull factors". Annals of Tourism Research. 21 (4): 844–846. doi:10.1016/0160-7383(94)90091-4.
  66. ^ "Push and Pull Motivation".
  67. ^ Jump up to:a b Chang, I.; Liu, C.; Chen, K. (2014). "The push, pull and mooring effects in virtual migration for social networking sites". Information Systems Journal. 24 (4): 323–346. doi:10.1111/isj.12030.
  68. ^ Kelly, Theresa (February 6, 2012). Empathy: A Quantum Approach – The Psychical Influence of Emotion. p. 101. ISBN 9781105482885. Retrieved 8 December 2014.
  69. ^ "Drive". Dictionary.com. Retrieved 22 March 2013.
  70. ^ Jump up to:a b c d e Schacter, D.L., Gilbert, D.L. and Wegner, D.M. (2009,2011) Psychology. 2nd ed. New York: Worth.
  71. ^ "How Does Drive Reduction Theory Explain Human Motivation?".
  72. ^ Kamlesh, M. L. (Jan 12, 2011). Psychology in Physical Education and Sport. Pinnacle Technology. ISBN 9781618202482. Retrieved 8 December 2014.
  73. ^ "Archived copy" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF)on 2014-10-27. Retrieved 2015-02-06.
  74. ^ Steel, Piers; C. König (2006). "Integrating theories of motivation" (PDF). Academy of Management Review. 31 (4): 889–913. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.196.3227. doi:10.5465/AMR.2006.22527462.[permanent dead link]
  75. ^ Jump up to:a b Steel, Piers (2007). "The nature of procrastination: A meta-analytic and theoretical review of quintessential self-regulatory failure" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 133 (1): 65–94. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.335.2796. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.65. PMID 17201571.
  76. ^ Atkinson, John; Norman Feather (1974). A Theory of Achievement Motivation (6 ed.). Krieger Pub Co. ISBN 978-0-88275-166-5.
  77. ^ Atkinson, John; George H. Litwin (1960). Achievement Motive and Text Anxiety Conceived as Motive to Approach Success and Motive to Avoid Failure. Bobbs-Merrill Company.
  78. ^ Atkinson, John; Joel O. Raynor (1978). Personality, Motivation and Achievement. Hemisphere Pub. Corp. ISBN 978-0-470-99336-1.
  79. ^ Xiang, P.; McBride, R.; Guan, J. (2004). "Children's motivation in elementary physical education: A longitudinal study". Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 75 (1): 71–80. doi:10.1080/02701367.2004.10609135. PMID 15532363.
  80. ^ McClelland, David (1953). The Achievement Motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  81. ^ Covington, M. V. (2000). "Goal theory, motivation, and school achievement: An integrative review". Annual Review of Psychology. 51 (1): 171–200. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.171. PMID 10751969.
  82. ^ Schultz & Schultz, Duane (2010). Psychology and work today. New York: Prentice Hall. pp. 121–122. ISBN 978-81-317-3370-7.
  83. ^ Jump up to:a b Entwistle, Noel (1988). "Motivational Factors in Students' Approaches to Learning". Learning Strategies and Learning Styles. Perspectives on Individual Differences. Springer, Boston, MA. pp. 21–51. doi:10.1007/978-1-4899-2118-5_2. ISBN 9781489921208.
  84. ^ Jump up to:a b c Fried, Yitzhak; Slowik, Linda Haynes (2004). "Enriching Goal-Setting Theory with Time: An Integrated Approach". The Academy of Management Review. 29 (3): 404–422. doi:10.2307/20159051. JSTOR 20159051.
  85. ^ Jump up to:a b "Theories of Motivation". analytictech.com.
  86. ^ Jaffe, Eric (2013-03-29). "Why Wait? The Science Behind Procrastination". APS Observer. 26 (4).
  87. ^ "Defense Mechanisms: Procrastination". hub.rockyview.ab.ca. Retrieved 2018-11-10.
  88. ^ "Lying and Procrastination". Psychology Today. Retrieved 2018-11-10.
  89. ^ "Procrastination". Psychology Today. Retrieved 2019-01-30.
  90. ^ Atkinson, John; David Birch (1970). The Dynamics of Action. New York: Wiley. ISBN 978-0-471-03624-1.
  91. ^ Kuhl, Julius; John W. Atkinson (1986). Motivation, Thought, and Action. New York: Praeger Publishers. ISBN 978-0-275-92096-8.
  92. ^ Atkinson, John; David Birch (1978). Introduction to Motivation. New York: D. Van Nostrand Company.
  93. ^ Reeve, J (2009). Understanding motivation and emotion (5 ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
  94. ^ Kassin, Saul (2007). Social Psychology. Wadsworth: Cengage Learning. ISBN 978-0618868469.
  95. ^ Weiner, Bernard (2000). "Interpersonal and intrapersonal theories of motivation from an attributional perspective". Educational Psychology Review. 12 (1): 1–14. doi:10.1023/A:1009017532121.
  96. ^ Jump up to:a b Schacter, Daniel. "Psychology". Worth Publishers. 2011. p.340
  97. ^ Elliot, Andrew J; Covington, Martin V (2001). "Approach and Avoidance Motivation". Educational Psychology Review. 13: 2.
  98. ^ Freud, Sigmund (2012). A General Introduction to Psychoanalysis. Renaissance Classics. ISBN 9781484156803.
  99. ^ Deckers, Lambert (2018). Motivation: Biological, Psychological, and Environmental. Routledge. pp. 39–41. ISBN 9781138036338.
  100. ^ "Priming | Encyclopedia of Psychology". Encyclopedia of Psychology. 2016-06-17. Retrieved 2018-10-13.
  101. ^ Jump up to:a b Elgendi, Mohamed; Kumar, Parmod; Barbic, Skye; Howard, Newton; Abbott, Derek; Cichocki, Andrzej (2018-05-30). "Subliminal Priming—State of the Art and Future Perspectives". Behavioral Sciences. 8 (6): 54. doi:10.3390/bs8060054. ISSN 2076-328X. PMC 6027235. PMID 29849006.
  102. ^ "Mere Exposure Effect | Encyclopedia of Psychology". Encyclopedia of Psychology. 2016-06-17. Retrieved 2018-10-13.
  103. ^ Jump up to:a b "Gordon W. Allport". psychology.fas.harvard.edu. Retrieved 2019-12-10.
  104. ^ Jump up to:a b author., Deckers, Lambert (2018-01-29). Motivation biological, psychological, and environmental. ISBN 9781351713887. OCLC 1022784633.
  105. ^ Jump up to:a b Jabr, Ferris. "Does Thinking Really Hard Burn More Calories?". Scientific American. Retrieved 2018-12-04.
  106. ^ Services, Department of Health & Human. "Fatigue". Retrieved 2018-12-04.
  107. ^ Mizuno, Kei; Tanaka, Masaaki; Yamaguti, Kouzi; Kajimoto, Osami; Kuratsune, Hirohiko; Watanabe, Yasuyoshi (2011-05-23). "Mental fatigue caused by prolonged cognitive load associated with sympathetic hyperactivity". Behavioral and Brain Functions. 7: 17. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-7-17. ISSN 1744-9081. PMC 3113724. PMID 21605411.
  108. ^ "Learned Industriousness – Dr. Eisenberger". classweb.uh.edu. Retrieved 2018-12-04.
  109. ^ Malone, Thomas (1997). "Is 'Empowerment' Just a Fad? Control, Decision-Making, and Information Technology". Sloan Management Review. 23 (38).
  110. ^ Markowitz, Linda (1996). "Employee Participation at the Workplace: Capitalist Control or Worker Freedom?". Critical Sociology. 22 (2): 89–103. doi:10.1177/089692059602200205.
  111. ^ Denison, Daniel R. (1990). Corporate Culture and Organizational Effectiveness. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
  112. ^ Miner, J.B. (2003). The rated importance, scientific validity, and practical usefulness of organizational behavior theories: a quantitative review. Acad. Manag.: Learn. Educ.
  113. ^ Tom Pisello (2004). "Managing IT According To A Hierarchy Of Needs". N/A. http://archive.webpronews.com/it/itmanagement/wpn-18-20040302ManagingITAccordingtoaHierarchyofNeeds.html "The well traveled theory by Abraham Maslow asserts that people are motivated by unsatisfied needs [...]."
  114. ^ Steinmetz, L. (1983) Nice Guys Finish Last: Management Myths and Reality. Boulder, Colorado: Horizon Publications Inc.
  115. ^ Steinmetz, L.L. (1983) Nice Guys Finish Last: Management Myths and Reality. Boulder, Colorado: Horizon Publications Inc. (pp. 43–44) https://books.google.co.nz/books?id=mR3UAAAACAAJ
  116. ^ Goldthorpe, J.H., Lockwood, D., Bechhofer, F. and Platt, J. (1968) The Affluent Worker: Attitudes and Behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  117. ^ Weightman, Jane. (2008) The Employee Motivation Audit: Cambridge Strategy Publications
  118. ^ Human Resources Management, HT Graham and R Bennett M+E Handbooks(1993) ISBN 0-7121-0844-0
  119. ^ Barnett, Tim, and Scott B. Droege. "Theory Z." Encyclopedia of Management. Ed. Marilyn M. Helms. 5th ed. Detroit: Gale, 2006. 914-916. Gale Virtual Reference Library. Web. 5 Feb. 2012.
  120. ^ Robbins, Stephen P.; Judge, Timothy A. (2007), Essentials of Organizational Behavior (9 ed.), Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall, archived from the original on 2009-06-14
  121. ^ Mann, Anna Marie. "Employee Recognition: Low Cost, High Impact". Gallup. Gallup. Retrieved 25 September 2019.
  122. ^ "Richard Hackman".
  123. ^ J.R. Hackman and G.R. Oldham. Work Redesign. Upper Saddle River, N.J.: Pearson Education, Inc, 1980; pp 78-80.
  124. ^ Katz, Ralph (2015). "Motivating Technical Professionals Today". Research-Technology Management. 48 (6): 19–27. doi:10.1080/08956308.2005.11657344.
  125. ^ Jump up to:a b Steel, Piers. Motivation: Theory and Applied. Boston, MA: Pearson Learning Solutions, 2012. Print. pp. 49
  126. ^ How Employee Recognition Programmes Improve RetentionArchived 2013-01-16 at the Wayback Machine CFO Insight Magazine, January 2013
  127. ^ Five mistaken beliefs business leaders have about innovationby Freek Vermeulen in Forbes, May 2011
  128. ^ Ormrod, 2003
  129. ^ Williams, Robert L.; Stockdale, Susan L. (2004). "Classroom motivation strategies for prospective teachers". The Teacher Educator. 39 (3): 212–230. doi:10.1080/08878730409555342.
  130. ^ Whyte, Cassandra B (2007). "An Additional Look at Orientation Programs Nationally- (reprint of 1986 article in same journal)". National Orientation Directors Association Journal. 15 (1): 71–77.
  131. ^ Vallerand, Robert J.; Pelletier, Luc G.; Blais, Marc R.; Briere, Nathalie M.; Senecal, Caroline; Vallieres, Evelyne F. (2016). "The Academic Motivation Scale: A Measure of Intrinsic, Extrinsic, and Amotivation in Education". Educational and Psychological Measurement. 52 (4): 1003–1017. doi:10.1177/0013164492052004025.
  132. ^ Harter, Susan (1981). "A new self-report scale of intrinsic versus extrinsic orientation in the classroom: Motivational and informational components". Developmental Psychology. 17 (3): 300–312. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.17.3.300.
  133. ^ Cordova, Diana I.; Lepper, Mark R. (1996). "Intrinsic motivation and the process of learning: Beneficial effects of contextualization, personalization, and choice". Journal of Educational Psychology. 88 (4): 715–730. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.88.4.715.
  134. ^ Whyte, Cassandra Bolyard (2018). "Effective Counseling Methods for High-Risk College Freshmen". Measurement and Evaluation in Guidance. 10 (4): 198–200. doi:10.1080/00256307.1978.12022132.
  135. ^ Lauridsen, K.; Whyte, C.B., eds. (1980). An Integrated counseling and Learning Assistance Center. New Directions Sourcebook. Jossey-Bass.
  136. ^ Fisher, K.; Marshall, M.; Nanayakkara, A. (2009). "Motivational orientation, error monitoring, and academic performance in middle childhood: A behavioral and electrophysiological investigation". Mind, Brain, and Education. 3: 56–63. doi:10.1111/j.1751-228x.2008.01053.x.
  137. ^ William T. O‘Donohue; Lorraine T. Benuto; Lauren Woodward Tolle, eds. (2013). Handbook of Adolescent Health Psychology. Springer. p. 376. ISBN 978-1-4614-6632-1.
  138. ^ Moen, Ross; Doyle, Kenneth O. (1978). "Measures of Academic Motivation: A Conceptual Review". Research in Higher Education. 8 (1): 1–23. doi:10.1007/BF00985853. JSTOR 40195071.
  139. ^ Symer, Matthew M.; Abelson, Jonathan S.; Yeo, Heather L.; Sosa, Julie A.; Rosenthal, M. Zachary (2018). "The Surgical Personality: Does Surgery Resident Motivation Predict Attrition?". Journal of the American College of Surgeons. 226 (5): 777–783. doi:10.1016/j.jamcollsurg.2018.02.007. PMID 29510202.
  140. ^ Jump up to:a b c d Maclellan, Effie (2005). "Academic achievement:The role of praise in motivating students" (PDF). Active Learning in Higher Education. 6 (3). doi:10.1177/1469787405057750.
  141. ^ Jump up to:a b Sansone, Carol; Morgan, Carolyn (1992). "Intrinsic motivation and education: Competence in context". Motivation and Emotion. 16 (3): 249–270. doi:10.1007/bf00991654.
  142. ^ Jump up to:a b c Katz, Idit; Shahar, Bat-Hen (2015). "What makes a motivating teacher? Teacher's motivation and beliefs as predictors of their autonomy-supportive style". School Psychology International. 36 (6): 575–588. doi:10.1177/0143034315609969.
  143. ^ Jump up to:a b Reeve, Johnmarshall (2009). "Why teachers adopt a controlling motivating style toward students and how they can become more autonomy supportive". Educational Psychologist. 44 (3): 159–175. doi:10.1080/00461520903028990.
  144. ^ Deci, Edward L.; Koestner, Richard; Ryan, Richard M. (2001). "Extrinsic rewards and intrinsic motivation in education: Reconsidered once again". Review of Educational Research. 71 (1): 1–27. doi:10.3102/00346543071001001.
  145. ^ Jump up to:a b Deci, Edward L.; Sheinman, Louise; Nezlek, John B. (1981). "Characteristics of the rewardee and intrinsic motivation of the rewardee". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 40 (1): 1–10. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.40.1.1.
  146. ^ Instructional Conversations in Native American Classrooms (1994). Center for Applied Linguistics: Online Digests (5).
  147. ^ McInerney, Dennis M.; Gayton Swisher, Karen (1995). "Exploring Navajo Motivation in School Settings". Journal of American Indian Education. 34: 3.
  148. ^ Pewewardy, Cornel (2002). "Learning Styles of American Indian/Alaska Native Students: A Review of the Literature and Implications for Practice". Journal of American Indian Education. 41: 3.
  149. ^ Wilfred Pelletier (1969). Childhood in an Indian Village. Institute for Indian Studies. Neewin Publishing, Toronto etc.
  150. ^ Maynard, A. E. (2004). "Cultures of teaching in childhood: formal schooling and Maya sibling teaching at home". Cognitive Development. 19 (4): 517–535. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.492.6959. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.09.005.
  151. ^ Jump up to:a b Greenfield, P. M.; Maynard, A. E.; Childs, C. P. (2000). "History, culture, learning, and development". Cross-Cultural Research. 34 (4): 351–374. doi:10.1177/106939710003400404.
  152. ^ Rogoff, Barbara (2011). Developing Destinies: A Mayan Midwife and Town. Cambridge: Oxford University Press.
  153. ^ Chavajay, Pablo (2002). "Schooling and Traditional Collaborative Social Organization of Problem Solving by Mayan Mothers and Children". Developmental Psychology. 38 (1): 55–66. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.38.1.55. PMID 11806702.
  154. ^ Lillemyr, Ole Fredrik; Søbstad, Frode; Marder, Kurt; Flowerday, Terri (June 2010). "Indigenous and non-Indigenous primary school students' attitudes on play, humour, learning and self-concept: a comparative perspective". European Early Childhood Education Research Journal. 18 (2): 243–267. doi:10.1080/13502931003784396.
  155. ^ Rogoff, B.; Paradise, R.; Mejia Arauz, R.; Correa-Chavez, M.; Angelillo, C. (2003). "Firsthand learning through intent participation". Annual Review of Psychology. 54: 175–203. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145118. hdl:10400.12/5953. PMID 12499516.
  156. ^ Rogoff, B (2012). "Learning without lessons: Opportunities to expand knowledge". Infancia y Aprendizaje / Journal for the Study of Education and Development. 35 (2): 233–252. doi:10.1174/021037012800217970.
  157. ^ Ames, P (2013). "Learning to be responsible: Young children transitions outside of school". Learning, Culture and Social Interaction. 2 (3): 143–154. doi:10.1016/j.lcsi.2013.04.002.
  158. ^ Gaskins, S (2000). "Children's daily activities in a mayan village: A culturally grounded description". Cross-Cultural Research. 34(4): 375–389. doi:10.1177/106939710003400405.
  159. ^ Correa-Chávez, M.; Roberts, A. L. D.; Pérez, M. M. (2011). Cultural patterns in children's learning through keen observation and participation in their communities. Adv Child Dev Behav. Advances in Child Development and Behavior. 40. pp. 209–241. doi:10.1016/b978-0-12-386491-8.00006-2. ISBN 9780123864918. PMID 21887963.
  160. ^ Mejía-Arauz, R.; Rogoff, B.; Dexter, A.; Najafi, B. (2007). "Cultural Variation in Children's Social Organization". Child Development. 78 (3): 1001–1014. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01046.x. PMID 17517018.
  161. ^ Ali, J.; Mcinerney, D.; Craven, R.; Yeung, A.; King, R. (2013). "Socially Oriented Motivational Goals and Academic Achievement: Similarities Between Native and Anglo Americans". The Journal of Educational Research. 107 (2): 123–137. doi:10.1080/00220671.2013.788988.
  162. ^ Paradise, R.; Rogoff, B. (2009). "Side by Side: Learning by Observing and Pitching In". Ethos. 37: 102–138. doi:10.1111/j.1548-1352.2009.01033.x.
  163. ^ American Intercontinental University. 5 Reasons College Students Should Make Time For Exercise. N.p., 14 Sept. 2012. Web. 27 July 2013.
  164. ^ Radoff, Jon. April 2011. Game On: Energize Your Business with Social Games. ISBN 978-0-470-93626-9
  165. ^ Radoff, Jon. "Game Player Motivations." May 2011. radoff.com Archived 2011-05-21 at the Wayback Machine
  166. ^ Popkin, Helen (June 1, 2010). "FarmVille invades the real world". NBC News.
  167. ^ Wan, C. S.; Chiou, W. B. (2007). "The motivations of adolescents who are addicted to online games: A cognitive perspective". Adolescence. 42 (165): 179–97. PMID 17536482.
  168. ^ Ryan, R. M.; Rigby, C. S.; Przybylski, A. (2006). "The motivational pull of video games: A self-determination theory approach". Motivation and Emotion. 30 (4): 344–360. doi:10.1007/s11031-006-9051-8.

Further reading[edit]

External links[edit]

Motivationat Wikipedia's sister projects

Library resources about
Motivation
show

Psychology

Authority control Edit this at Wikidata

Categories:

Navigation menu

Interaction

Tools

In other projects

Print/export

Languages

Edit links

  • Wikimedia Foundation
  • Powered by MediaWiki



Discussion

Do you want to join discussion? Click here to log in or create user.